Placeholder Image

字幕列表 影片播放

  • Before I start, I just want to thank the key people involved in this talk. This is Aiyana

  • Willard. She was a graduate student when I was a graduate student at the University of

  • British Columbia. If I understand correctly, she’s coming here as a postdoc next year.

  • Stephanie Kramer’s a graduate student of mine at the University of Oregon, and of course

  • thank you, Cristine, for organizing this. It’s great so far. Cristine assigned me

  • this topic [Psychological and social consequences of religious (dis)belief] to talk about. I

  • wasn’t sure exactly what she wanted, so I changed it to this [Moral consequences of

  • religious (dis)belief], which is something I’ve been thinking about a lot recently

  • and running a number of analyses on. Over dinner, I realized that this was a little

  • irrelevant to what were talking about today, and so I woke up at 5 AM and wrote a new talk.

  • I’m actually going to go with the first talk, but if anybody’s interested in what

  • the new talk was about, which was about how stereotypes about Christians being unintelligent

  • and unscientific end up undermining their interest in science and pushing them out of

  • the field. That’s another topic I can talk about, either in the question-answer period

  • or you can come find me afterwards. Today I’m going to talk about this question [“Does

  • God make you good?], because this question seems like one people are interested in. People

  • have been interested in it for a very long time. Back in the time of Socrates, we know

  • at least back then they were debating this question. Twenty-five centuries ago, if we

  • fast-forward to the tail-end of the Enlightenment, you have common arguments being made. This

  • is Voltaire. He said, “This sublime system is necessary to man. It is the sacred tie

  • that binds society, the first foundation of holy equity, the bridle to the wicked, the

  • hope of the justif God did not exist, it would be necessary to invent him.” We fast-forward

  • a little further. This is the late great Christopher Hitchens. I like Christopher Hitchens a lot,

  • but he’s a little off the mark in this statement. So he says, “We keep on being told that

  • religion, whatever its imperfections, at least instills morality. On every side, there is

  • conclusive evidence that the contrary is the case and tat faith causes people to be more

  • mean, more selfish, and perhaps above all, more stupid.” Now he’s right about this

  • first bit. We keep hearing about this. But he’s wrong about everything after that.

  • There is not conclusive evidence about religion making people mean or selfish. There are many,

  • many anecdotes that you can pull to make that claim. But in terms of there being evidence

  • for this, there is evidence, it doesn’t lean in that direction, it’s not nearly

  • conclusive. That’s the topic that I’ve undertaken to try to answer, to try to find

  • an answer to that questiondoes God make you good? It’s a nice question, because

  • you have this nice alliteration betweenGodandgood”, and they kind of look like

  • homographs of each other. It’s also a question that’s fraught with ambiguities. It really

  • matters what you mean by good, and it matters what you mean by God, and it sort of matters

  • what you mean by make. Let’s focus on this one right now. I think this is possibly the

  • most important. You do find that if youre looking at how religiosity affects how people

  • act morally, there are systematic differences in between what religious and nonreligious

  • people think is moral. In particular, one of the recent and popular theories on moral

  • pluralism has been moral foundations theory, which fractionates our moral concerns into

  • 5 categories. Harm and care, so issues of compassion, issues about suffering. Justice

  • and fairness, that’s another concern. Concerns about authority, respecting people who are

  • in authority. Loyalty to the group and purity and disgustanother concern for what’s

  • moral and what isn’t. If you look at how this varies across the religious spectrum,

  • you see that the first two there’s agreement on. There’s not much variance in how much

  • religious people versus nonreligious people believe that this is an important concern.

  • But on the other 3 is where you see the difference. It’s the difference you’d expect if youre

  • familiar with research on political conservatism and moral foundations. These results exist

  • after you control for political conservatism, and in many cases theyre actually stronger

  • than the political conservative differences between conservatives and liberals. There

  • is disagreement on what it is to be moral between these groups. Nonreligious people

  • alsothis is work by Jared Piazzatend to base their morals on utilitarian consequentialism.

  • So what are the benefits and costs that happen based on the action that you should or should

  • not enact. Theyll be more likely than religious people to agree to a statement like this:

  • Should you break a promise if it may lead to a greater good or prevent further suffering?

  • Theyll say yes, if it’s going to create greater good, sure let’s do that. Religious

  • people are going to be much more deontic with this. Theyre going to feel that they should

  • not break the rule, even if the consequence of not breaking the rule is greater suffering.

  • There are these important foundational meta-ethical differences. But there are areas of agreement.

  • Those are areas in which we can make direct comparisons. Things like generosity or not

  • cheating, charity, helping, or volunteerism. These are all situations that are not pitting

  • 2 different moral foundations, say freedom vs. equality or justice vs. mercy, against

  • each other. Instead youre pitting selfish behavior against non-selfish behavior, or

  • what you could call prosociality vs. antisocial selfishness. It’s on this measure, prosociality,

  • this constellation of moral behaviors that we can actually look at differences across

  • the religious spectrum. So we conducted a meta-analysis of as many of the studies as

  • we could find that’s been done on this. We pulled this all together. This includes

  • 31 studies, over about 4 decades, with almost 31,000 participants. We included studies if

  • they had people reporting their religiosity and if they had some measure, either a self-report

  • measure or a behavioral measure, of that constellation of moral behaviors that we talked about beforeprosociality.

  • The reason that we split these apart is because there are limits to some of these measures.

  • So self-report measures, I think most of you guys are going to be familiar with the limitations

  • of asking people how moral they are. Here’s an example. So this is a common self-report

  • measure. This is the satisfaction of life scale, and if you say that the conditions

  • of my life are excellent, that youre satisfied with your life, I will generally take your

  • word for that. Imagine if you switched this to the satisfaction with yourself scale, and

  • then you made a claim that I’m a good mother, I’m not racist, I’m charitable and a moral-abiding

  • person. Those are situations where maybe I don’t want to take your word as much because

  • you could be self-inflating. You could be overemphasizing how good you are compared

  • to how good you actually are. So, we also included behavioral measures, and we made

  • a distinction between these. Behavioral measures are measures that social psychologists and

  • experimental economists have concocted to measure actual behavior in which there’s

  • a direct cost for the participant to actually demonstrate that they are pro-social in this

  • situation. One of these examples is they create a spontaneous helping situations. Someone

  • will drop a bunch of papers accidentally, and theyll see how many participants in

  • the study actually pick them up. Other examples are economic games. A common one is the Dictator

  • Game. A lot of you will have heard of this Dictator Game. Basically it’s very simple.

  • You take 2 participants into your lab room. You put them into separate rooms. They don’t

  • meet each other, they don’t have reputational concern with each other, and then you give

  • one of them $10. You give the other one, the receiver, nothing. Then you say to the dictator,

  • the person who got the $10, you can dictate how much each of you gets. The other person

  • got nothing; do you want to split the $10 that you have? So you could split it 50-50,

  • which would be the equitable thing to do since you were both randomly put into these roles.

  • You could be more selfish. You could only give them $1, or you could do what most people

  • in the game do, which is to give them nothing and keep the $10 themselves. So these are

  • examples of behavioral measures. When we put all these togetherlet me just orient you

  • to this graph. This is a forest plot of the 31 studies that I was talking about. When

  • the squares and the lines go towards that side, that indicates that there’s a negative

  • association between religiosity and pro-social behaviors. So religious people in that study

  • ended up being less pro-social than the nonreligious people. On the other hand, if it’s passing

  • that line, that means it’s significantly more pro-social for religious people. There’s

  • a positive relationship between religiosity and pro-sociality. So what you can see here

  • is there is small but consistent positive relationship, in terms of pro-sociality with

  • religious people. If you were to break it down between these self-report measures, these

  • behavioral measures, there you see a difference. There are the self-report measures. It’s

  • a correlation of about .2, in terms of the self-report measuresquestions about how

  • much do you volunteer, how much do you give to charity. And if you look at the behavioral

  • measures, theyre actually 0. There’s no hint of an effect. There’s actually a

  • substantial difference between what is reported when youre asked how pro-social you are

  • and, when you test in the lab, how pro-social religious people actually demonstrate they

  • are. So there are a couple reasons that could explain this discrepancy, as far as I can

  • think of. One, is the one that might immediately be most obvious, that the self-reports aren’t

  • actually accurate. You should trust the behavioral measures over the self-reports. What’s happening

  • here is that religious people are inflating reports of their actual prosocial behavior.

  • Everybody tends to inflate these. The expectation is that maybe religious people are fudging

  • a little bit more than nonreligious people. There’s another explanation, which is that

  • the behavioral task should not be tested. These are not actually reflective of any real

  • differences that might exist outside of the lab. There may actually be a pro-social religious

  • advantage that were not tapping into with these artificial tasks that were looking

  • at. I think both of these are true to some degree. Let’s start with the first one.

  • There has been a bunch of work showing that religious people do score higher on measures

  • of social desirable responding. They tend to indicate that theyre better than they

  • actually are, more than do nonreligious people. There have been a couple meta-analyses recently.

  • This one demonstrated that strongly. It makes the case that religious people tend to be

  • self-enhancing. This paper actually makes it really interesting argument. If you could

  • think of the intuitive explanation, the one that may have occurred to you guys is something

  • like religion might make people more self-enhance. Or there could be some sort of personality

  • factor that makes you both religious or self-enhancing. These guys argue something separate. They

  • argue something more like this, that it’s actually self-enhancement, the tendency to

  • enhance yourself, that makes you more religious. They say that there is something called the

  • self-enhancing personality and that, and I quote, “people strategically use an ingenious

  • array of means to satisfy the self-enhancement motiveReligion is a prevalent and important

  • such aspect. Peoplewill be likely to capitalize on it for satisfying the self-enhancement

  • motive.” So people who are going to want to aggrandize themselves find a way to do

  • so in religion. They present a bunch of compelling evidence for that. I’m convinced about this

  • to some degree. Another point that needs to be taken into accountthis is not incompatible

  • with thatis if you ask people if theyre religious and if you ask them how pro-social

  • they are, it’s not that they may only be inflating how much theyre pro-social. They

  • may also be inflating how much they say they are religious. We can’t get super accurate,

  • implicit measures of what people actually believe, but we can see if people inflate

  • their religious attendance compared to how much they actually do attend religious services.

  • So this is a recent study. What they did was they asked people how much they attend, but

  • they asked people in 2 separate ways. They randomly assigned people to be actually asked

  • in person; face-to-face to somebody to whom theyre going to have to respond, or online

  • where youre not actually talking to anybody. The assumption is that people are actually

  • going to be more honest when theyre revealing this online, but when theyre confronted

  • with a person theyre going to hedge a bit. That’s what they found. So when you look

  • at what people say when asked online, you have about 31% saying that they go weekly

  • or more. When theyre asked by telephone, that number increases to 36% and the percentage

  • of people who say they never go actually falls from 43% down to 30%. So theyre less willing

  • to admit when theyre actually directly talking to somebodyOh, I don’t go to

  • church.” There have been a number of studies like this. One of my favorites is from the

  • 90s, where they actually just did a head count of everybody who said they went to church

  • last Sunday and compared that to actually the number of people who were there. They

  • did this across the United States. This is for a few of the cities. When you actually

  • ask people how many of you went to church last Sunday, these are the rates you get.

  • When you actually count how many people showed up, it’s much lower. In some places, it’s

  • less than half as much. So people do seem to be inflating their actual religious attendance.

  • What that means is that when were measuring what might be a relationship between religiosity

  • and prosociality, we might actually be measuring a relationship between your tendency to inflate

  • how much you go to church and your tendency to inflate how much prosocial behavior you

  • enact in. So really what youre doing is youre just finding that people who fudge

  • on one thing fudge on the other. I guess that’s interesting. Let’s tackle the other possibility,

  • that our behavioral tasks are not actually telling us what we want them to be telling

  • us. Here, I think it’s good to consider the situations in which religious people might

  • actually in the real world have a prosocial advantage. It’s not in these cold, sterile

  • lab rooms when that would emerge. It’s when theyre actually enmeshed in the community,

  • or thinking about their religious beliefs, that are actually getting them to engage in

  • prosocial behavior. So it doesn’t look so much like this, a cold, sterile lab room,

  • but more like this. When youre reminded of religion, when youre thinking of religion,

  • that’s the situation religious people might actually be more prosocial. There have been

  • a number of studies that have recently tried to create that religious situation by reminding

  • people in the moment of their religiosity, arousing those thoughts and seeing how that

  • affects their prosocial behavior. It’s used a number of priming techniques. They will

  • in various waysexplicit, obvious, and implicitarouse thinking of religiosity in the lab, in the

  • moment. Field studies have done this with environmental prime. So theyll do experiments

  • outside of churches to see how that affects people compared to outside of other buildings.

  • I’ll just give you 1 example of this. This is an early study that we ran. This used an

  • implicit priming technique with scrambled sentences. For those of you guys who don’t

  • know how this implicit priming technique works, basically you get a bunch of sentences or

  • word scrambles like this. You cross out 1 word, and then you rearrange the remaining

  • 4 words to make a sensical sentence. You change that tohe tied his shoes”. That’s

  • something you would see in the control condition. In the condition were actually trying to

  • arouse thoughts of religion, you embed keywords in some of the sentences. So here, you can

  • try this if you want, what would it be? You droperadicateand you can makeshe

  • felt the spirit”. In one of the studies, well talk about this later, in one of the

  • conditions rather, we included a third condition where we actually instead of priming god,

  • we primed secular institutions of justice. So they went to court. Then we had them play

  • the Dictator Game, the anonymous Dictator Game that I mentioned before. These are the

  • results. This is just comparing in the control condition, non-theists compared to theists.

  • You see there is a bit of a directional difference, but there’s no significant difference between

  • these 2, which is consistent with what we found in the meta-analysis. This is a behavioral

  • task. You don’t see a difference based on religiosity. But our question is what happens

  • when you put them in the priming situation. Here’s what they found in the first study.

  • When people are just in the control condition, no religion is aroused, out of $10 people

  • share on average just under $2 with the other person. So theyre pretty selfish. When

  • you prime religion, that shoots up almost to that $5 level of equity. In the other conditionthis

  • was with community members who tend to be a little more generous in control conditions

  • than studentsit was still pretty low, and you still saw the same increase. So in this

  • case you see that religious priming does increase generosity. Now you say, well this is an artificial

  • task itself in the lab. This is not how people come across religion in their real lives.

  • I think that were actually primed all the time with religion. This is the view out the

  • window of my apartment in Eugene, Oregon. That’s a church with some hieroglyphics

  • on it. That’s what I stare at every day when I look out the window. A couple of years

  • ago, I was teaching at Abu Dhabi, and this was the view outside of my window there. There’s

  • one mosque, but you can also see another mosque over there and another mosque over there.

  • So there were 3 mosques that were directly outside of my window. For any of you who have

  • spent any time in Muslim-majority countries, youll know that the most beautiful and

  • hard-to-ignore reminder of religion is the call to prayer, which goes off 5 times a day

  • and blared across the entire city. A number of researchers have actually used that call

  • to prayer as a naturally occurring religious prime and found consistent results with what

  • we saw. Mark Aveyard, who’s in the UAE, showed that when the call to prayer was playing

  • in the background, people were less likely to cheat on this task that may be tempting

  • to cheat on. Eric Duhaime has this unpublished study where he actually went to Morocco, to

  • Marrakech, and had people play a Dictator Game-type task with shopkeepers there. He

  • then measured when the call to prayers were going on in the background and when it wasn’t.

  • What he found was that when it wasn’t happening, the shopkeepers were reasonably likelyabout

  • 60% of the timethey engaged in the fully selfless option in the Dictator Game. They

  • gave all the money to the charitable recipient. When the call to prayer was going, this went

  • up to 100%. Every single one of the shopkeepers, when that was happening, gave all the money

  • away. So it had this profound prosocial effect. In America, a couple of economists have actually

  • looked at a naturally occurring prime in just the day of Sunday. Just it being Sunday to

  • a lot of people activates religious thinking. What they found was that when you have a website

  • which is prompting people to give to charity, on Sundays, you have religious people giving

  • a lot more than nonreligious people. But on every day except Sunday, there is no difference.

  • And so that’s actually, again, that’s consistent with what we saw before, right?

  • So when there’s no prime, when youre just in a control condition, there’s no

  • difference between religious people and nonreligious people, only when religion is activated do

  • you see this difference. We ran another meta-analysis where we took 25 of these priming studies.

  • They had to be assigned to either a control condition or a priming condition, and there

  • had to be again either a behavioral or self-report measure of prosociality. All but 2 of these

  • were behavioral measures. So there were 25 studies, just under 5,000 participants. What

  • you find is that there is this significant effect where priming people with religion

  • increases their prosocial behavior. Now, a subset of these studies, 11 of them, actually broke

  • people down between religious people and nonreligious or less religious people. And so you can actually

  • look at the difference in how these primes affect them. What you find is that the primes

  • do affect religious people, but they have no effect on nonreligious people, which is

  • kind of what you expect. There’s this interaction between the religious personality, or the

  • religious disposition, and the religious situation. This is actually in that study that I mentioned

  • before; it’s what we found there as well. So if you just look at in the control condition

  • between our theistic and our non-theistic subjects, in the control condition they again

  • didn’t differ. When there was the religious prime, you do see that it affects the religious

  • people, but it does not affect the non-theists in this situation. But you recall that I mentioned

  • that there was this prime of secular institutions, and you see what happens there is that it

  • actually increases how much people give in both cases. And so that’s a particularly

  • effective way of stimulating prosocial behavior. Some of you are probably thinking that this

  • may explain a question you have in the back of your head, which is that hold on, some

  • of the least religious places in the world are some of the most cooperative and prosperous

  • places. I think this gives an explanation as to why. There are these different institutions

  • that, when they emerge, they can regulate cooperation, they can encourage people to

  • act prosocially and not cheat, so that religion no longer has to. In a way, these take the

  • place of those religious institutions that were previously responsible for encouraging

  • prosociality. I think that’s why if youthis is what you could call a secular alternative

  • for what was previously done—I think why you see that one of the strongest negative

  • predictors of religiosity across countries is the presence and trust in these reliable

  • secular institutions in the rule of law. So you see it’s a very very actually stark

  • relationship there where, the higher the trust in the rule of law, the less religious you

  • are. So let’s recap then. Does god make you good? Let’s go back to the original

  • question. Well yes, you have to admit that it really does, at least temporarily. It acts

  • in the same way that food acts with satiety. You eat, but then you get hungry again. It

  • has to be constantly refreshed so as you keep remembering to be pro-social. But do you need

  • god to be good? The answer here is clearly that no, that’s not the case, so long as

  • you have something else. There are other routes to pro-social behavior. That’s all I want

  • to say.

Before I start, I just want to thank the key people involved in this talk. This is Aiyana

字幕與單字

單字即點即查 點擊單字可以查詢單字解釋

B1 中級 美國腔

宗教(不)信仰的心理和社會後果。 (Psychological and Social Consequences of Religious (Dis)belief)

  • 173 13
    Jack 發佈於 2021 年 01 月 14 日
影片單字