Placeholder Image

字幕列表 影片播放

  • - A very important idea

  • that people are often unaware of in 'false effect'

  • that we have two completely different ways

  • of seeing the world, two different neural networks we access

  • when we're perceiving things.

  • So what this means is

  • when we first sit down to learn something,

  • for example, we're going to study math,

  • you sit down and you focus on it.

  • So you focus, and you're activating task-positive networks,

  • and then what happens is you're working away,

  • and then you start to get frustrated.

  • You can't figure out what's going on,

  • and what's happening is you're focusing

  • and you're using one small area of your brain

  • to analyze the material, but it isn't the right circuit

  • to actually understand and comprehend the material.

  • So you get frustrated, you finally give up,

  • and then when you give up and get your attention off it,

  • it turns out that you activate a completely different set

  • of neural circuits that's the default mode network

  • and the related neurocircuits.

  • So what happens is you stop thinking about it,

  • you relax, you go off for a walk, you take a shower,

  • you're doing something different

  • and in the background, this default mode network

  • is doing some sort of neural processing on the side,

  • and then what happens is you come back

  • and, 'voila!'

  • suddenly the information makes sense.

  • And in fact, it can suddenly seem so easy

  • that you can't figure out

  • why you didn't understand it before.

  • So learning often involves going back and forth

  • between these two different neural modes,

  • 'focus mode' and what I often call 'diffuse mode,'

  • which involves loose, neural resting-states.

  • You can only be in one mode at the same time,

  • so you might wonder, 'Is there a certain task

  • that is more appropriate for focus mode or diffuse mode?'

  • The reality is that learning involves going back and forth

  • between these two modes.

  • You often have to focus at first

  • in order to sorta load that information into your brain,

  • and then you do something different,

  • get your attention off it,

  • and that's when that background-processing occurs.

  • This happens no matter what you're learning,

  • whether you're learning something in math and science,

  • you're learning a new language, early music, a dance,

  • even learning to back up a car.

  • And think about it this way,

  • here's a very important related idea-

  • here's a very important related idea-

  • and that is that when you're learning something new,

  • you want to create a well-practiced neural pattern

  • that you can easily draw to mind when you need it.

  • So this is called a 'neural chunk,'

  • and chunking theory is incredibly important in learning.

  • So, for example, if you are trying to

  • learn to back up a car,

  • when you first begin, it's crazy, right?

  • You're looking all around-

  • do you look in this mirror or this mirror,

  • or do you look behind you?

  • What do you do?

  • It's this crazy set of information.

  • But after you've practiced a while,

  • you develop this very nice sort of pattern

  • that's well-practiced, so all you have to do is think,

  • "I'm gonna back up a car."

  • Instantly, that pattern comes to mind

  • and you're able to back up a car.

  • Not only are you doing that,

  • but you're maybe talking to friends, listening to the radio.

  • It's that well-practiced neural chunk

  • that makes it seem easy.

  • So it's important in any kind of learning

  • to create these well-practiced patterns.

  • And the bigger the library of these patterns,

  • the more well-practiced,

  • sort of deeper and broader they are

  • as neural patterns in your mind,

  • the more expertise you have in that topic.

  • And chunking was first sort of thought of or explored

  • by Nobel Prize-winner Simon,

  • who found that if you're a chess master,

  • that the higher your ranking in chess,

  • the more patterns of chess you had memorized,

  • so you could access more and more patterns of chess.

  • So research began developing,

  • and what they found was that

  • the better your expertise at anything,

  • the more solid neural patterns,

  • what I called neural chunks, you have.

  • So, for example,

  • if you might know how to do mathematics very well;

  • well, you've got certain patterns related to multiplication,

  • and you've practiced quite a bit with them,

  • and so you can pull them instantly to mind,

  • and, likewise, division.

  • And then you go higher, so you've got calculus,

  • you've got the concept of the limit,

  • you've got integrals, derivatives.

  • And you've practiced with each one of those enough

  • so that it is almost like backing up a car.

  • All you have to do is think,

  • "Oh, I've got to take this derivative,"

  • and, boom, off you go.

  • You're taking the derivative, and it seems very easy to you.

  • So a challenge that we've had is,

  • for a long time, particularly in mathematics education,

  • it was felt that if you practiced too much,

  • that it would kill your creativity,

  • and that's actually not true.

  • You want to do the right kind of practice,

  • where you're interleaving and doing one technique,

  • and then trying that with another technique.

  • You don't want to just be doing the same thing

  • over and over again.

  • But practicing by-

  • here's a good way to practice developing a chunk:

  • Let's say that you've got a homework assignment,

  • that you've got this homework problem,

  • and it's a really difficult homework problem.

  • So, what do you tend to do?

  • Well, you do it and you turn it in.

  • That is the equivalent of you have just sung a song one time

  • and thinking that you know how to sing that song beautifully

  • in front of an audience.

  • Well, it doesn't work that way.

  • A good thing to do

  • when you're learning something that's difficult

  • is find, in math, key problems

  • and then see if you can work it cold.

  • If you can't, take a peek at whatever hints you need

  • to be able to finish working it.

  • Then maybe a little later, try working it again cold,

  • without looking at the answer.

  • And maybe you go further.

  • The next day, try it again.

  • Go a little further and practice it.

  • What you're trying to do is to develop the same patterns

  • that you would develop

  • if you practiced singing a song a number of times.

  • And if you do this with key problems in math,

  • or if you're learning a language,

  • key conjugation patterns, for example,

  • then those patterns become automatic.

  • So, for example with your problem,

  • after several days of practice,

  • you find you've worked it out enough times by pencil

  • that when you just look at the problem,

  • you can step through all the solution steps in your mind.

  • You've created a valuable chunk,

  • and so then when it comes test time,

  • and you've got maybe five, ten of these key problems,

  • so you can just look at 'em

  • and know what you're supposed to be doing.

  • Suddenly when you're taking that test,

  • you can pull this chunk up and connect it with this chunk,

  • and solve new problems you haven't seen before.

  • And it's really, really powerful technique is to realize

  • that all learning involves

  • getting these neural chunks together.

- A very important idea

字幕與單字

單字即點即查 點擊單字可以查詢單字解釋

A2 初級 美國腔

如何將信息轉化為情報| 芭芭拉·奧克利(How to turn information into intelligence | Barbara Oakley)

  • 139 5
    chen 發佈於 2023 年 07 月 23 日
影片單字