Placeholder Image

字幕列表 影片播放

  • Let me tell you about Robber's Cave. In 1954, a group of 11 boys, all about 12 years old,

  • were invited to a special summer camp in the deep woods of southeastern Oklahoma, at a

  • place called Robber's Cave State Park. None of the boys knew each other, although they

  • all came from similar backgrounds. They spent their days bonding over things like games

  • and swimming and treasure hunts, and in no time, they formed a tight friendly group.

  • They even came up with a name for themselves: the Rattlers.

  • But soon they began to notice something. No, not a guy in the woods with a hockey mask,

  • there was another group of boys also 11 of them, also the same age, that had been staying

  • at the other end of the park the whole time. The Rattlers never interacted with these other

  • boys, so they didn't know that those kids were also spending time bonding over games

  • and swimming and treasure hunts, and that they'd come up with a name for themselves,

  • too: the Eagles. But the Rattlers didn't like the look of the Eagles, oh, no, they didn't

  • like them using their baseball diamond or their dining hall.

  • And the feeling was mutual. It didn't take long for each group to start complaining to

  • the camp's counselors about the other gang, and eventually, they both said that they wanted

  • to set up a contest to determine once and for all which group was better. The counselors

  • were only too happy to comply, because as I'm sure you've figured out by now, those

  • counselors were actually researchers.

  • The man who set up what would be remembered as the Robber's Cave Experiment was Turkish

  • American social psychologist Muzafer Sherif. He was interested in what it would take for

  • rivals to overcome their differences and resolve their conflicts. Specifically, Sherif wanted

  • to test something called Realistic Conflict Theory. He hypothesized that conflict happens

  • when you combine negative prejudices with competition over resources, and the boys at

  • Robber's Cave were well on their way to proving him right.

  • Over the next couple of days, the Rattlers and the Eagles competed against each other

  • for prizes in a series of games, like tug-of-war and foot races, and soon, what started as

  • your basic trash talking and taunting and name-calling morphed into fist-fights, thefts,

  • and raids on each others' cabins. But then their dynamics changed or were changed for them.

  • After the games were over, the researchers integrated the groups and gave the kids shared

  • goals that they could only achieve through cooperation. The tide quickly turned. All

  • 22 boys worked together to move a stalled truck that was carrying their food, they took

  • care of a partially felled tree that was deemed a danger to the camp, they collaborated in

  • setting up tents, even though they weren't given complete sets of equipment. While isolation

  • and competition made enemies of the strangers, shared goals and cooperation turned enemies into friends.

  • Over the past 39 weeks, we've learned a lot about ourselves, our emotions and our personalities,

  • how our minds can get sick, how we can help them get well again, why we can do vicious

  • things and then turn around and act like heroes. So maybe it's fitting that we wrap up this

  • course by looking at a couple of opposing forces that some consider the very definition

  • of human nature: aggression and altruism. Conflict and cooperation. You might think

  • of it as the psychology of war and peace, or simply, what we can all learn from a bunch of 12 year olds.

  • In psychology, aggression is defined as "behavior intended to hurt or destroy someone, something,

  • or even yourself." People aggress, as psychologists say, in all kinds of ways, verbally, emotionally,

  • and physically, and for lots of different reasons: out of anger, to assert dominance,

  • or as a response to fear. But that's just a glimpse into why someone might become aggressive.

  • Where does the aggression actually come from?

  • Like a lot of behaviors we've talked about it, it seems to emerge from that familiar

  • combination of biological factors, like genetic, neurological, and biochemical influences,

  • and our environment and experience. In terms of genetic influences, studies of twins, and

  • yes, Crash Course Psychology might have been called Crash Course Studies of Twins, showed

  • that if one identical twin has a violent temper, often the other one does, too, but fraternal

  • twins are much less likely to be so similar.

  • Neurologically speaking, no single area of the brain controls aggression, but certain

  • areas like the limbic system do appear to facilitate it. Research on violence and criminality

  • has also revealed a link between aggression and diminished activity in the frontal lobes,

  • which play a vital role in impulse control.

  • And finally, our aggressiveness can be influenced by our own biochemistry, hormones like testosterone

  • and glucocorticoids and pheromones have all been implicated in animal models of aggression.

  • It's a little trickier in humans, it's a lot trickier in humans, but it's highly likely

  • that our hormones are intimately linked with feeling and showing aggression.

  • Obviously, aggression isn't just about biology. Psychological and cultural factors also play

  • an important role, as does the power of the situation. For example, there's the Frustration-Aggression

  • Hypothesis, the simple idea that people become aggressive when they're blocked from reaching

  • a goal. To demonstrate, consider the not-very aggressive sport of baseball. There's a study

  • that analyzed 44 years worth of baseball stats, and focused on the more than 27,000 incidents

  • when a pitcher hit a batter with a ball. It turned out that this was most likely to occur

  • if the pitcher was frustrated by a recent home run or if one of his own teammates had

  • been hit by a pitch in the previous inning.

  • But we also learn aggression by watching others. Like, if you grew up watching your parents

  • throw popcorn and jeering lewdly at their most hated soccer team, you might have learned

  • something from their behavior. So combine all of those biological factors and funnel

  • them through a particular person with a particular history in a particular situation, and you

  • can begin to see how aggression can have many roots that grow together.

  • Thankfully, though, humans are more than their bad tempers. While some things in people will

  • leave us annoyed and angry, others breed friendship and affection. So yes, there are positive

  • topics in social psychology, like altruism, our selfless, even self-sacrificing regard

  • for the welfare of others. This could be something as simple as jumpstarting a stranger's car

  • or as heroic as running into a burning building to save someone. But if being altruistic is

  • so awesome, why aren't we all that way all the time? Or maybe the better question is,

  • why do we ever do anything selfless, like, what's in it for us?

  • In the late 1960s, social psychologists Bibb Latane and John Darley conducted a series

  • of experiments examining when and why we help others. In one experiment, they placed a subject

  • in a room, sometimes alone, sometimes with two other subjects, and sometimes with two

  • actors posing as subjects. Then, they simulated an emergency by filling the room with smoke

  • and waited to see if the subject would do anything to alert the others or help themselves.

  • If the subject was alone, they'd report the smoke 75% of the time. But subjects in a group

  • of three only spoke up 38% of the time. And when they were stuck in the room with two

  • oblivious actors, only 10% of the participants said anything to the others. Darley and Latane

  • found that people typically helped others only if they noticed the incident, interpreted

  • it as an emergency, and then finally, assumed responsibility, and all of these things were

  • much more likely to occur if a person was alone, while the presence of others deterred

  • the person from helping.

  • This kind of diffusion of responsibility referred to as the bystander effect, can weaken our

  • instinct for altruism. The bystander effect is a bit like the concept of social loafing

  • that we talked about. If you're around other people, it's easier to think that someone

  • else is going to pick up the slack or in this case, come to the rescue.

  • When people do decide to help others, they may do it for a number of reasons. One perspective

  • is that we tend to help others mainly out of self-interest. By this thinking, helping

  • really isn't altruistic at all, and instead, our actions boil down to a sort of cost-benefit

  • analysis. Like, maybe we'd turn in a lost wallet because we're hoping for a reward or

  • we pitch in on a project at work because we think we'll get recognized and promoted by

  • our bosses. Social psychologists contextualize these kinds of examples in the broader theory

  • of social exchange. When it comes to doing things for other people, we're always trying

  • to maximize our personal rewards while minimizing our costs. But social exchange doesn't have

  • to be as selfish as that, it can also mean that we act altruistically because we expect

  • that the people we help will go on to help others, so if we give someone a hand changing

  • a tire, maybe they'll stop next time they see someone else, maybe even us, broken down

  • on the side of the road. You might know this concept, sometimes it's called the norm of

  • reciprocity, sometimes it's called paying it forward.

  • And then there's the social responsibility norm, which is the simple expectation that

  • people will help those who depend on them, like any parent can expect to give more help

  • than they're going to receive from young children. That's just part of being a parent. Naturally,

  • the world would be a delightful place if altruism were the standard for human behavior, but

  • then, psychology wouldn't be nearly so interesting.

  • In some ways, you might say that what fuels conflict is the opposite of altruism: self-interest.

  • Social psychologists view conflict as any perceived incompatibility of actions, goals,

  • or ideas. That could mean two nations fighting over a border, sparring religious or political

  • groups, or you and your boo fighting over whose turn it is to do the dishes.

  • And in a weird conundrum of human behavior a lot of conflicts arise from what psychologists

  • call "a social trap," where people act in their own short term self-interest, even though

  • it takes a toll on the larger group and on themselves over the long-term. You see this

  • kind of thing all the time on an individual scale, like in a crime movie, when a criminal

  • just betrays all of his criminal friends to get the big payout, it doesn't turn out very

  • well for him in the end. But on a larger scale, you can find social traps taking their toll

  • on the environment, like when we poach elephants to sell their ivory or cut down old growth

  • forests to make a quick buck in the lumber market. Either way, when self-interest succeeds

  • in wrecking the collective interest by, say, depleting some limited resource, it becomes

  • easy to start viewing our neighbors as competitors, taking us right back to the ingroup vs. outgroup

  • mindset that we all know causes big problems.

  • So as long as there's self interest, there's gonna be conflict.

  • But before you get all down on humanity, remember those Robber's Cave boys. They were ready

  • to go full-on Lord of the Flies before shared goals forced them to cooperate and ultimately,

  • make peace. The power of cooperation to make friends of former enemies is one of the most

  • hopeful areas of psychological research. If greed and self-interest can destroy the world,

  • perhaps cooperation can save it.

  • Today, you learned about the Robber's Cave experiment and what it taught us about realistic

  • conflict theory and how shared goals can overcome conflict. We looked at the physical and environmental

  • triggers of aggression, and the frustration-aggression hypothesis. You also learned about altruism,

  • the bystander effect, and when we are more or less likely to help a person in need, and

  • also about the social exchange theory, the reciprocity norm, the social responsibility

  • norm, and social traps.

  • Thanks for watching, especially to all of our Subbable subscribers who make Crash Course

  • possible. To find out how you can become a supporter, just go to Subbable.com, and please

  • remember to go to YouTube.com/CrashCourse and subscribe for more future Crash Courses

  • in the future.

  • This episode was written by Kathleen Yale, edited by Blake de Pastino, and our consultant

  • is Dr. Ranjit Bhagwat. Our director and editor is Nicholas Jenkins, the script supervisor

  • and sound designer is Michael Aranda, and the graphics team is Thought Cafe.

Let me tell you about Robber's Cave. In 1954, a group of 11 boys, all about 12 years old,

字幕與單字

單字即點即查 點擊單字可以查詢單字解釋

B1 中級 美國腔

心理學速成班 #40(Aggression vs. Altruism: Crash Course Psychology #40)

  • 12 2
    erinfong7212 發佈於 2022 年 02 月 18 日
影片單字