字幕列表 影片播放
Professor Paul Bloom: So what we're doing today is
continuing on the theme of emotions.
"Emotions" is a two-part lecture and we're continuing
along certain themes. I want to begin by responding
to a question which was raised in the last class concerning
smiling and nonhuman primates. It was a very good question.
The issue was: we know that humans have
different sorts of smiles to convey different sorts of
information. The question was,
"do nonhuman primates, like chimpanzees or gorillas or
gibbons, have the same many sorts of smiles?"
So, I contacted the world's expert on smiling,
who did not return my e-mails. So, I contacted the second
world's expert on smiling who told me that the answer is "no,"
that primate--nonhuman primate smiles actually correspond
almost entirely to appeasement smiles.
They're "don't hurt me" smiles. They're equivalent to the "coy
smile" that we saw on humans. But that nonhuman primates do
not use smiles for greetings; there's no equivalent to the
"greeting smile" or "Pan Am smile";
nor do they use them as genuine expressions of happiness.
There's no equivalent to the "Duchenne smile."
That's as far as I know. If the world's expert gets back
to me and says something different, I'll keep you posted.
Another thing. Going back to the beginning
theme of the class, what we started--just to
review, we talked about the different functions of emotions.
And then we talked about smiling and facial expressions.
And then we turned to some--to a nonsocial emotion,
the case of fear. And then we shifted to social
emotions. And we talked about social
emotions towards kin and the special evolutionary reasons
that would lead them to evolve. And as we were ending,
we were talking about the relationship between an animal
and its children, particularly in cases like
humans and birds and mammals where there tends to be a close
relationship with our children. We invest in quality,
not quantity. I might produce very few
children in my life. And my evolutionary trick then
is to focus very intently on them and make sure they survive.
If I were to produce 100 children, I could stand to lose
a few, but if I just produce five in my lifetime or two or
one, they become very precious to me.
And so, the story of the evolution of a species like us
involves a long period of dependence and deep,
deep bonds between the parent and the child.
And that's part of what I talked about,
how parents respond to children.
And I want to begin this class by giving an illustration from a
documentary about parental response to children,
but I want to give it in a species that's not us.
And here is why. I'll explain why with an
analogy. I have a friend of mine who
studies the psychology of religion.
He studies why people hold religious beliefs.
And he tells me that when he's talking to a non specialist,
somebody not in the field, he doesn't ever tell them,
"Yeah, I'm really interested in why people believe in the Bible
or why people light the candles on Sabbath or why people go to
church" because these are religions that people around
here hold, and if you tell people you
study them they'll sort of be puzzled, "why would you want to
study something like that" or offended.
If you want to talk about the psychology of religion to an
audience like this, what you do is you start with
the exotic. So, you start by talking about
people who put butter on their heads.
Dan Sperber talks about a culture where the men put butter
on their heads in the summer. And it kind of melts and that's
part of--one of the things that they do or--you talk about a
culture that believes in spirits or that trees can talk.
You say you're studying it and they say, "Oh,
that's interesting. I wonder why they believe that?"
And you use that as a way to look at more general facts that
exist even in our culture. You use the fact that we don't
take the exotic for granted as a way to motivate the scientific
study of things we do take for granted.
And this is, of course, true more generally.
This was the point in the William James quote when he
talked about things that are natural to us and noticed that
some very odd things are equally natural to other species.
And it's true, I think, in particular when we
talk about things like the love we have for our children.
So, one way to look at the love we have for our children
scientifically, isn't to look at it head-on,
because the love we feel towards our own children feels
sacred, it feels special, but look at it in other species.
And so, one of the nicest illustrations of this is the
Emperor penguin, which was--which--whose
childcare and mating practices were dramatized in a wonderful
movie called "March of the Penguins."
And this is interesting because they had this incredibly
elaborate and quite precarious system of generating and taking
care of offspring. So, I want to show you a brief
clip of the movie to illustrate some parts of this.
What they do at the beginning, which is not--which leads up to
this, is they take a very long trek from the water to their
breeding grounds. Their breeding grounds is--are
protected from the wind and they're on a firm piece of ice
so they could hold the whole pack.
They do the breeding there and it's there that the eggs are
created. So, this is where the movie
begins at this point. "March of the Penguins" was the
second best--second most popular documentary of all time,
beaten only by "Fahrenheit 9/11."
And people responded to it in different ways,
which are informative when we think about the generalizations
you could make from animal behavior to human behavior.
Some conservative commentators saw this as a celebration of
family values, such as love and trust and
monogamy. Some liberals,
who hate everything that's good and true, [laughter]
responded by saying, "Well, yeah,
they're monogamous for one breeding season.
It's a year. Then they go and find another
mate. If you add it up,
it's pretty slutty." [laughter]
I think more to the point, people were impressed and
stunned by the rich and articulate and systematic
behavior that these animals were showing.
Plainly, they didn't pick it up from television,
movies, culture, learning, schooling,
and so on. To some extent,
this sort of complicated behavior came natural to them.
And it's understandable that some proponents of intelligent
design, or creationism, pointed to this as an example
of how God creates things that are deeply, richly intricate so
as to perpetrate the survival of different animals.
From a Darwinian standpoint, the Darwinian would agree with
the creationist that this couldn't have happened by
accident, this is just far too
complicated, but would appeal to the--to this as an exquisite
example of a biological adaptation,
in particular a biological adaptation regarding parental
care to children shaped by the fact that children share the
parents' genes and so parents will evolve in ways that
perpetrate the survival of their children.
Then there's the other direction, which is how children
respond to parents, how the young ones are wired up
to resonate and respond in different ways to the adults
around them. And we quickly talked about
some different theories of this. And I'll just review what we
talked about last class. Babies will develop an
attachment to whoever is closest.
They'll usually prefer their mothers because their mothers
are typically those who are closest to them.
They'll prefer her voice, her face, her smell.
It used to be thought that there is some sort of magical
moment of imprinting that when the baby is born,
the baby must see his or her mother and "boom," a connection
is made. If the baby doesn't,
terrible things will happen with attachment later on.
This is silly. There is no reason to believe
there's some special moment or special five minutes or special
hour. It's just in the fullness of
time babies will develop an attachment to the animal that's
closest to it. They will recognize it as,
at an implicit level, at an unconscious level,
as their kin. Well, how does this work?
How does the baby's brain develop--come to develop an
emotional attachment to that creature?
Well, you remember from Skinner that operant conditioning could
provide a good answer to this. And this is known as the
"Cupboard Theory," which is babies love their moms because
their moms provide food. It's the law of effect.
It's operant conditioning. They will approach their
mothers to get the food from them.