字幕列表 影片播放 列印英文字幕 CARL SAFINA: All right, well, we're going to talk about basically the subtitle of this book. This book came out two days ago, so it's brand new. And we're going to start in a kind of familiar place. Many of us have animals at home, and we've often asked ourselves these questions, right? How many people have asked themselves if their cat or dog really loves them, or just wants food, or whatever, right? That's pretty common. And we think, well, it's impossible to know. But is it really? Is it really impossible to know what's going on in the mind and the heart of our pets? Well, in a way, no and in a way, yes. What's really going on in those minds is the question. Another question is-- it's not showing up very well there-- how are they like us? But I don't like that question so much. It's an inescapable question because they are in many ways like us. But when we say, how are they like us, we put the attention back on us. And us is our favorite story. We like to talk about ourselves. And what we're really supposed to be doing here is asking, how are they like us or not? Who are we here on Earth with is really the point. And what is going on in these minds that are our co-voyagers on planet Earth? Is there any way to get into the mind of an elephant, for instance, or any other kind of creature? Well, I think there are actually several really good ways of seeing in. You can look at their brain and their mind. You can look at their body, at the logic of their behavior, at their evolution. So the first thing is if we're interested in minds is to know where is the mind? In many parts of human history, people have thought the mind is in the heart or the mind is in the soul. The mind is a disembodied spirit that is our eternal presence in the universe, all these different kinds of things. So first of all, where is the mind? And a hint is that if you have a heart surgery, it's not going to damage your mind. But if you have brain surgery and something goes wrong, it's going to damage your mind. Your mind comes from your brain. The mind is in the brain. And we might say, oh, we have no idea if animals are experiencing anything. But we use their experience all the time. For instance, if we want to know if cosmetics are going to sting us, we test them on the eyes of rabbits to see if they sting the rabbit. So if they sting the rabbits, the rabbits are having exactly the same experience of pain, the sensation of stinging, as people have. When dogs are depressed, or if dogs have obsessive-compulsive disorder, they respond to the same drugs in the same way that we give to people-- antidepressant drugs. Why? Because the part of the brain that gets depressed and the chemicals that create depression or obsessive-compulsive disorders are exactly the same. They're not experiencing an analogous problem. They're having the same problem in the brain of a dog. Or even as low as down below vertebrates on the evolutionary timescale, or before vertebrates, something like a crayfish, you can make crayfish develop anxiety by giving them little electrical shocks and making them feel nervous and tense. And so they retreat into their burrows. They stop exploring. They won't eat. You give them the same anti-anxiety drugs that work on people, the crayfish relaxes, comes out of its burrow, and starts exploring again. So these are ways we can manipulate minds with different kinds of tests and drugs to see if they respond the same way ours do. And that's a good way in. Now you can say, well, we can see brains, but we can't see the mind. And that, in a sense, is a fair enough statement. I mean, it's true, right? But I can't see your mind. You can't see my mind. It's always true. But where is our mind from? Where is the human mind from? The human mind is from the minds of non-humans that were here before us. When evolution created-- in a sense-- humanity, humanity evolved from something. We had to use the parts that were already in stock and make a few tweaks. And the parts that were in stock are the highly developed brains of the other mammals from which we evolved. So if you look at a mouse brain, and you look at a human brain, it's a very similar, very recognizable kind of thing. The human brain looks different. It's got lots of convolutions in the forebrain. That's where a lot of thinking happens in humans-- human thinking. If you compare our brain with a chimpanzee brain, you see basically that we are apes, so it's not too surprising that the human brain is basically a very big chimpanzee brain. So it's not logical to think that the human brain only does things that only humans do and nothing that happens in any other brain. It's just completely illogical. We're all very insecure, we human beings. We can soothe our insecurity with the thought that at least we have the biggest brain, right? Except, uh-oh, there's a dolphin brand. It's not only much bigger. It has way more convolutions than ours. It's doing something with all of those neurons and all of those networked connections. We can see the working of the mind in the logic of behaviors of other animals, the fact that their responses and emotions make sense to us. We don't see-- well, we'll go through a few, OK. We say these albatrosses here, they're dancing in courtship. And why do we say they're dancing in courtship? Because that's a very recognizable thing. It happens at the same time that humans dance in courtship. It's something that we do, pre-bonding and pre-mating-- same with them. We look at elephants like this. Now, elephants don't do this if they're surrounded by all kinds of dangers. And they don't do this if they're famished. They do this if everything is cool and nice, and they're relaxed. And the parents are still a little bit on guard while they let the babies lie down and not worry about anything. That totally makes sense to us, and it's totally appropriate to the situation. So we can tell by the logic of their behavior, something about what's going on in their minds. They are protective and parental, like we are protective and parental, whether they are humans or elephants or mammals in water. The differences between us are mostly the outer contours and a few internal tweaks. So one analysis is only humans have human skeletons. But it's not true that only humans have skeletons. And only humans have human minds, but it's not true that only humans have minds. When help is needed, help is provided. They're not trying to eat their baby. They're trying to help it to its feet. I love this picture. This is the cover of the book, actually. That's a newborn elephant that has never stood up before. And its mother and two cousins are helping it to its feet for the first time. When animals are very young, they're very curious. They don't know what things like egrets are. They check everything out, because they have to learn what's in their environment. And many animals have to learn just about everything. Baby elephants don't even know that lions are dangerous. They have to be taught by their parents and by example that lions are dangerous, that bees can sting you, and these kinds of things. But when we're young, we do lots and lots of exploring. And as we get a little older, and we know what's going on in our environment, and what we have to do, we tend to be less playful than when we were children. We tend to do less exploring. You can see that in others. And when something goes wrong, or something might be dangerous, you see a very different set of behaviors. And you see them paying attention in very different ways. When it's time to relax and have fun, they relax, and they have fun. Now, no one would look at this and say, I have no idea if the baby is frightened out of its mind, right? It's not what you'd say. You can see that they're just having a nice time in the water. And that not all of their behaviors are driven by the need to survive in the next minute, that there are other things that help us to survive. A lot of these deep motivations like love and like bonding, these things, we don't just do it in the minute it's needed. We develop relationships before these things need to come into play. So the love and bonding of family members is something that provides ongoing benefits in many ways. But we don't think, OK, let's see, I'm going to get ongoing benefits in many ways, so I better love my siblings, and my parents, and my family members. That's just not how our minds are equipped. We just feel these motivations, and then they do what they are supposed to do, what they're evolved to do. Albatrosses have these bonds and maintain these pair bonds in ways that are a bit unusual among birds, actually even a bit unusual among mammals. Because their pair bonds are very, very long-lasting. They can live many decades in the wild. They mate with the same mate every year. And if a mate dies, it usually takes them two or three years to court and re-mate. So you see the deep maintenance of these bonds in them In a way that I find quite touching and really quite beautiful. All right, there are other things that are unexpected, I guess. Only unexpected because our assumption and what we've been taught is that animals have very, very simple minds, and they're not really aware of much. And they just do things without thinking about it, without feeling about it. But it turns out that if you broadcast the recorded conversations of tourists, farmers who farm away from where the elephants live, and pastoralist herders who walk around with spears and get into trouble with elephants around water holes, and fairly frequently hurt the elephants, that the elephants ignore the conversations of the tourists. They ignore the conversations of the farmers. And when they hear the conversations of the herders broadcast out of hidden speakers, they react with alarm. The family bunches up, and they run away. They can actually tell the difference between human languages. And they know which ones are dangerous to them. They react exactly the same way to clothes that are worn by those three different groups, that are placed on trails where elephants can encounter them. And that's been shown with experiments. So let's get a little bit deeper into a question here that is fundamental, which is, can animals think? Well, first of all, the shortest answer is, of course. Because humans are animals. And human thinking is animal thinking. But another thing is, we need much better definitions of these words that we tend to use. So what do you mean by think is the first question that you should ask. What do you mean by grieve? What do you mean by empathy, and all these questions that are discussed with different animals. Thinking is the processing of information that's coming in through the senses in ways that help you make decisions. So a jaguar that is trying to sneak up on a tapir or a peccary, a wild pig, is thinking. He's making calculations. They know that the intended prey will run away if it detects them, and it is trying to sneak up on it. So that's thinking. Somebody who is aiming an arrow at a target is thinking. And somebody who is entertaining a proposal of marriage should be thinking, although perhaps not always. Consciousness is another thing that the definitions are all over the map. And some people think that consciousness means that you can plan years ahead. And consciousness means these complicated things that-- I don't really think that that's what it means. Because if you are given total anesthesia, the difference between conscious and not conscious is induced by the anesthesia. Consciousness means you're experiencing sensations. It's the thing that feels like something. That's what consciousness is. So a motion sensor senses, but it doesn't experience sensations. Dogs playing on the beach experience sensations. That's the difference between an unconscious reaction and consciousness. I think consciousness comes into play in evolution at the level where animals need to make decisions about things. So if you simply cut your leg, that's just a physical thing. And lots of unconscious processes start happening immediately and automatically. The blood starts clotting. The cells begin to heal the cut. The immune system kicks in to prevent infection. So those are physical things if you cut your leg. But if your leg hurts when you cut it, then you know you're conscious. So that's just consciousness. There are, in a sense-- now this is just something that I made up to try to tease out different aspects of consciousness-- these four interrelated processes of consciousness. We detect the world through our senses. We get information in. Then if we think about that information, if we evaluate that information, that is thinking. If we just have an emotional response to the information, that's emotion. If it changes our mood to see something-- we may see something that frightens us. We may see something that makes us jealous. And then these moods or these thoughts give us motivations and urges to act. Empathy is often talked about as something that is uniquely human. And when you are trying to write a book about what animals think and feel, you read a lot about humans. And you come across constant claims about what makes us human and what things are uniquely human. And most of them are wrong. One of the things that supposedly makes us uniquely human is empathy. Well, empathy is the ability to feel with your companions and to match their moods. If you get scared, I get scared. You feel and look sad, I get sad or concerned. That's empathy. It means your mind is able to match the mood of your companions. This is not something new that just started when people arrived, and suddenly, here I am, a human being, suddenly, oh, empathy. The oldest form of empathy is something really important and really basic, which is, if you're with companions, and one of them is suddenly startled and frightened, you cannot wait around saying, wow, what got into you? Why did you just run away? Why is that? Because the predator will nail you. So empathy and the mind's ability to match moods instantly with companions is something very, very old. And the oldest kind of empathy is called contagious fear or fear contagion. There are different kinds of empathetic responses. Contagious yawning is the weirdest one, because I don't even know what yawning is. I don't think anybody even knows what yawning is. And yet, it's contagious. It's very strange. And I think you can tease out different kinds of empathy as well. So there's basic empathy-- feeling with another. You match moods. A little bit away from that, and a little distant from that is what I call sympathy. You say, oh, my grandmother died. I don't know your grandmother. It doesn't make me sad to know your grandmother died. But it makes me a little sad to know that that makes you sad. So that's a kind of empathy I call sympathy. And then if you see something happening, and you are moved to act, I call that kind of empathy compassion. It's like sympathy and the urge to help. And the motivation to help and do something is what I call compassion. So you see somebody who's homeless, and you want to buy them a sandwich. Or you want to sign a petition to save the whales, or anything like that, where your mood is affected, and you are urged to act. That I call compassion. Human empathy is far, far from perfect. This thing, we like to exalt ourselves and say, oh, we're the most empathetic. Well, maybe and maybe not all the time. So on the left there is a bunch of dogs bundled up in a cage for a dog-eating festival in China recently. And on the right-- I'm not sure how well you can see that-- but that is a man in a cage in Indonesia. He is a Burmese man. He is a slave. This was a picture taken this past March when hundreds of Burmese slaves were found being forced to work on Thai fishing boats. Buy shrimp that has been wild-caught in Thailand, chances are pretty good that slaves have caught it, because human empathy is not really so great all the time. A thing that I find interesting is that people who know essentially nothing about the science of animal behavior know one thing, and that is, you should never use anthropomorphism. Everybody knows this tricky word, anthropomorphism, which is projecting human thoughts and emotions. In the behavioral sciences, doing such a thing, and attributing human thoughts and emotions to animals, has been for a few decades, a total career killer. It's absolutely not allowed in academia. And journalists write about this all the time. And journalists try to be very professional by saying, oh, we can't anthropomorphize the animals. But since their minds and their imperatives are very similar to ours because we came from what they have, I think it's actually a very helpful first step. And then if you know a little bit something about animals, you're not likely to make many mistakes. For instance, these two elephants are consorting, right? That's a male on the right and a female on the left. Actually, the female had a baby just as the book was going to the publisher. And if you saw them, and you were really projecting, you might say, oh, look at them. They're in love. But if you talk to any elephant researcher, it's well-known that elephants actually don't have romantic love. They do find sex very exciting, and that is very obvious. This one in the front has just made it and run back to her family. Her facial glands-- they have glands on their faces, kind of like we have glands in our armpits. And when they get very emotional about anything, when they're happy or frightened, at a heightened emotion, those glands stream. So she's just made it. Her family is reassuring her. And they're sniffing her, and they're checking her out. But there's no pair bond between her and the male. The male provides no parental care. So it is obvious that they do find sex thrilling, but sex for them is not always coupled with romantic love. I don't know if that sounds familiar to any of you, but I'm not going to ask, OK? So the main thing that evolution shows is that life is on one continuum. There are no sharp breaks. Everything that we see in people, we see the beginnings of in other animals. Some things that we see in people, we see other animals that have it far, far better developed. Many other animals have superhuman response times, superhuman strength, superhuman homing abilities, things like that that we just don't have. And we have some things that they don't have. But it's all on a continuum. All the way back to worms, the chemicals that worms make that create mood and motivation are some of the same chemicals that create mood and motivation in human brains. Even plants make some of those chemicals. And it's unclear whether any plants actually have any sensations at all. They don't have a nervous system like animals do, so there's some speculation on that. And many people say no, and some people say, yes, probably. But especially among animals, there are no sharp breaks. So we recognize when an animal looks hungry or thirsty, we say, oh, it's hungry and thirsty or it's tired. And then when they're happy, we say, oh, we have no way of knowing. This is the science of animal behavior. And actually, that is just not scientific. It's really a bias. So denying that they have thoughts and feelings helps us to retell our own favorite story. What is our favorite story, do you think? Who wants to take a guess at this? Yes. AUDIENCE: Animals are here for us to use. CARL SAFINA: That's one of our main stories. It's not our favorite story that I was thinking of. AUDIENCE: We're the endpoint and the goal of evolution. CARL SAFINA: We're the endpoint of evolution, the goal of evolution. And we are absolutely special and absolutely different and better than everything else. And part of the reason we tell ourselves that story is so we can think that all animals are here for us to use, which makes it easier to make decisions about what we're going to do to them. So those two things combined, I think, help reinforce us. It just makes it easier for us to continue to think and believe those things. What about love? Do animals feel love? This is a baby humpback whale, a nursing age humpback whale that washed up in East Hampton a couple of summers ago. And the night that that whale washed up, the lighthouse keeper at Montauk, 15 miles away, said that she heard incredibly mournful sounding whale sounds coming from the ocean all night. And she said, nobody would believe me if I told you that. But I mean, she is the lighthouse keeper at Montauk Point. So I do believe her. It sounded like there was a whale out there looking for its lost baby. So what do we mean by this word love? Here's another definitional problem for us, right? Many of us would say that we love our parents. We would say that we love our children. We also say we love that movie. We love ice cream. We love having a glass of wine before bed. Some people love fighting. So if we use that word, that word that's so important, if we use that word to cover all kinds of things that we like-- I love ice cream-- then it is clear and inescapable that animals love. Animals that care for one another, your pets that have a relationship with you and seek contact and comfort from you, that that is love. It may not be exactly the same kind of love that you feel when you talk about how you love your spouse, or your lover, or your children. But the way you love your spouse is not the same exact way that your spouse loves you, right? So love is not all one thing. And I think we can safely say that animals do often-- those animals that really experience these social bonds and these protective bonds-- experience love. Now, this guy here, that elephant on the right, his name is Philo. And he's a 15-year-old juvenile male in Kenya. And that's the last photograph taken of him, because four days later, that was him on the lower right with a bullet hole in his brain. Elephants are maybe the most famous animals with regard to their responsive grief. When an elephant goes down, the family always rushes to it and tries to lift it and aid it. And if an elephant dies, family and friends hang around, often for days, and will revisit the bones, often for years. And when they revisit the bones, they usually touch the parts that they knew best in life-- the tusks, if there are any tusks left, the teeth. Because when elephants greet, they usually greet in a trunk to mouth greeting. It's kind of a combination handshake, hug, and kiss that they do with their trunk. Grief is something that I've noticed and read about, not only in elephants, where it's really pretty well-known and well-documented, but in wolves. A researcher told me about a female wolf whose mate had died. And she had pups that were less than a year old. And she immediately left. She left her pups. It was winter. She went on a two-week trip on the highest, bleakest parts of Yellowstone National Park, where there were no tracks of prey. There was nothing she was doing there. She was just wandering, kind of aimlessly. And two weeks later, she came back, and she rejoined those pups. This sounds silly, but it was very interesting to me. I had a pair of ducks, and these ducks were inseparable. We had two ducks and four chickens. They were inseparable. And the drake died. And the female spent weeks wandering around the yard, quacking, and calling, and looking, and calling, and looking. And you could really see that she was taking a lot of time out of her normal routine. It wasn't her normal routine at all. She wasn't eating in the same way. She wasn't going to the same spots and resting in the same way. And a woman named Barbara J. King, who wrote a book on grief, provided I think a very helpful definition of that word, which is that grief happens when a companion is lost, and the individuals who have lost the companion change their routine. They go off their feed. They look around. They're trying to find the companion, that that's grief. And that's pretty much what we do. And our grief takes different forms also. For some of us, we might miss a day or two of work, and then we're kind of back to it. Go to the wake and come back. And others of us, our entire lives are upended and never really the same. It's similar to other families of animals that I've seen, families of wolves, and families of elephants, where if something happens to the adults, they get shot. They die really prematurely. The whole trajectory of the family is changed forever. We don't really tend to know these things because watching animals and knowing them is very, very new. The first people who ever systematically watched wild animal behavior are still alive-- Jane Goodall, and Cynthia Moss, and one or two other people who've been doing that for about 40 years, since the 1960s. That's as far back as we go to understand anything about the family lives of individuals. And they can tell you, I've known that elephant for 40 years. I knew who her mother was. I know what happened to her sisters, I know what she did during the last drought to keep the family alive. It's really incredible, but it's not something that has seeped deep down into general human knowledge. All right, this I mentioned. There's a lot of grief in the world in and around elephants nowadays. That woman is Daphne Sheldrick, and she runs the very famous elephant orphanage in Nairobi, Kenya. Kenya is only one country in Africa, and that's only one orphanage. And they're constantly besieged right now by orphans because there's this war against elephants, really, going on. I mean, it's basically an extermination campaign going on because of how out of control the ivory market has gotten again. But she will tell you, as she told me, that an elephant can actually die of grief. And many of these young elephants who've watched their families in the turmoil of getting demolished, come in very, very traumatized and take many months to settle down, and then years to try to repatriate them in these not very natural artificial families that they are creating back in the wild. I had the extreme good fortune of being allowed to go out with them on these walks every day. They take these little orphan baby elephants, let them out of their pens in the morning. They know the keepers very well. They bond with each other and with the keepers, and they walk around. And in a few minutes, you're in the bush. You can't see them most of the time. You can kind of hear them. If the keeper wants them to go somewhere else, we're going to go down this hill now, they just call. All the babies come out of the bush. They follow them down. And it's really moving to see and feel the capacity that these creatures have. And just because we're just discovering it, doesn't at all mean it's new. It's not at all new. It's many, many millions of years old. And it's the lives that they have and the vivid lives that they lead. And as I mentioned, nowadays, all these elephants literally have a price on their head. It's very, very remarkable that this guy-- his name is Tim-- that he is still alive. Around Roman times, when Europeans first took an interest in ivory, elephants lived everywhere from the Mediterranean to the bottom of the Cape of Good Hope, except for the bleakest parts of the Sahara Desert. The African elephant covered all of Africa. And you can see how its range has shrunken since 1980 and is just fracturing into splinters now. That is the geography of an animal that we are driving to total extinction. In another part of my travels for the book, I went to Yellowstone Park to look at wolves. Wolves are really, really incredible animals. They're very, very much like us. They live in family groups with a breeding pair and the youngsters of several different ages, usually one to three years old. And then after that, the youngsters leave and try to find and establish their own packs. These two adults from one of the main packs-- this one was a really, really famous wolf. Lots and lots of people watched her and photographed her. And she was very well-known. They left the park and were almost immediately shot. And their whole family kind of fell apart. The alpha male was left with no mate. That guy on the top right was his brother. Basically, his hunting companions and the ones who held down the territory were gone. He had no mate. Two new males came in. He lost his family, basically. And then this daughter, who was the most precocious one in the pack, got ganged up on by these two sisters, who wanted to be the mates of the two new males. And they forcibly ejected her from her own family permanently and wouldn't let her back in. Now, I knew that wolves live in packs. And I knew that the packs were families-- that I knew. And I knew that, in that way, they were a lot like us. But what I had no idea about was the turmoil that would happen in the family when something happened to the parents-- the coalitions, the vendettas, the politics. It was really all too human. So that one that got run out, she had to eventually leave the park, and she got shot. And this guy, who was the alpha male, the breeding male that lost everything-- he lost his family, he lost his mate, he lost his territory, and it was winter, and he was not young-- he, incredibly enough, against all odds, he is still alive. That's him sitting on a rock. He had just been calling to another female that he was trying to draw out of another pack to try to take her as his mate and start a new pack. So anyway, that was him when I was there, and this is him now. And he's still alive, which I think is quite remark-- it makes me feel really good that he survived. We like survival stories. What do you think that is? AUDIENCE: A dog? CARL SAFINA: You think it's a dog. Who thinks it's a dog? Who thinks it's a wolf? All right some dogs, some wolves. OK. Well, it's a full-blooded wolf. And an interesting thing about dogs is that before people understood evolution, but they started applying Latin names to animals, the dogs were called Canis familiaris, the familiar canid. And wolves were Canis lupus. But now we know enough about their genetics to realize that dogs are so minutely differentiated from their wolf ancestors, that their Latin name got changed back to wolf. So the dog's Latin name is now Canis lupus-- wolf-- familiaris. So it's a wolf, but it's our wolf. Domestication doesn't mean a tame, wild animal in captivity. It means an animal or a plant genetically changed from its wild type. Almost all animals that are domesticated are domesticated for tameness first. And it turns out, weirdly, that the same genes that make animals tame and tractable also create certain similar physical changes. They create floppy ears, blotchy-colored coats, coats of different textures, curled tails, passive personalities. The same genes, not a suite of different genes. So dogs show a lot of that. Farm animals show a lot of that. Who else shows some of that? AUDIENCE: Foxes. CARL SAFINA: Foxes. OK. Well, that gets to an interesting story, which is that most of this stuff that's known about what's called domestication syndrome is known because of a 40-year experiment with foxes done in Russia, where they selected them only for tameness. The ones that were tame and friendly, they let breed. And the ones that weren't, they basically killed. After a few generations, they got tame foxes that wag their tails, seek their humans, and have floppy ears, blotchy coats, and curly tails. These are hand raised wolves. They act remarkably like dogs. They have not been bred for 10,000 years around people. These are our wolves at home, our two wolves. They're not fighting, they're playing. They're good friends. Other aspects of domestication include a lot of sex outside the breeding season that does not result in offspring, passive behavior, conformity, reduced brain size, and neoteny. Neoteny means you carry juvenile traits way into maturity, right? That's what it means. So, can anybody think of any other animal that does all of this? AUDIENCE: Humans? CARL SAFINA: Humans. Right, humans. Now, why would humans have those traits? If you look at the bottom thing there, farm animals, we brought animals onto farms to live a passive, settled crowded life, and be able to conform and get used to it and do OK there. And we inadvertently had to do exactly the same thing to ourselves. So in the process of domesticating animals, we slightly, at least, domesticated ourselves. We are also like big juveniles, many of us, as you can clearly see by all the themed sections of this building. [LAUGHTER] All right. In the interest of time, and because we stopped late, I think I'm going to stop there. Because I really want to have some discussion and some questions. So I think I said we stopped late. We started late. So I'm going to just stop right there. And anybody have any questions about animals? Animal thinking, animal emotions, objections to anything I said? We can just take it all on. Yes? AUDIENCE: I wanted to know your opinion about studies where scientists assess animal intelligence and give equivalents, like, oh, a dog's equivalent to a three-year-old, like an octopus is equivalent to an eight-year-old. What's your opinion on that? CARL SAFINA: Yeah, what's my opinion of studies that equate the intelligence of animals with humans at certain ages. Well, in a way, they're interesting. And in a way, they're the wrong question. And I came to see that it's the wrong question because I asked Cynthia Moss, who has been studying elephants for 40 years. This, I thought, was the big question. I said, what has watching elephants for 40 years taught you about human nature? And I thought that was a really deep question, and it was going to be so interesting to talk about. And she said, I'm not really interested in that. I'm interested in elephants. I want to learn about elephants. Comparing an elephant or a raven's performance on a puzzle to a human toddler doesn't interest me at all. And I was very taken aback by that. I also felt kind of stupid for asking that question, considering her response. And I thought about that. It took me a few days for me to really do a full mental reset on that question. The problem with comparing a raven to a toddler or the communication ability of a chimpanzee to a small child is not that it tells you nothing, but that it doesn't ask the question that lets you get the full answer. It's always about us. Tell me, what could you learn about the ability of a raven to solve a puzzle by comparing it with a child? You learn, again, that humans are better at solving puzzles than ravens. But you don't really learn a lot about what ravens do in their complicated, vivid lives that they lead among their friends and their rivals in familiar territories, how they seek status, how their status gets deposed over the course of their life, how their life follows the arc of a career. You don't learn any of that about them by comparing them to a three-year-old. So the whole question of what intelligence is, is a very fraught definitional question anyway, because there are just different kinds of capacities. And usually with humans, we think of intelligence, more or less, as the ability to solve novel problems. And many humans have a pretty good ability to solve novel problems. But who was more intelligent, Henry Ford or Pablo Picasso? Does intelligence mean anything at most levels of performance and existence? And many animals do things that we can't do at all. But if we can do anything, we give ourselves an enormous amount of credit. Like we have the ability to learn music, for instance, just to think of one thing off the top of my head. We can learn music, and we can learn how to play instruments that other people have invented for us to play and make music. We just have a musical ability. We don't have such a great homing ability. We can't home for thousands of miles across open ocean just based on our perception of the Earth's magnetic field, and things like that. So fish and birds that can do that, we just discount that. We don't say, oh, my god, they're brilliant at this. They are geniuses compared to us. We never say that. We just say, oh, who cares? Well, we can play an instrument, or we can solve a problem, or something like that. There have been experiments done with ravens, with certain kinds of puzzles, where the ravens can figure it out. And dogs and toddlers are left, in the words of one researcher, "not even realizing there was a puzzle to solve." So to me, it just isn't really all about us, and it's not all about now. And we have to get outside of ourselves. Every time, we're always looking at the world through our eyes, which of course is the normal starting place. But when you look from the inside out, you only can see an inside out world. When you can get outside yourself, like the famous iconic first photograph of Earth from space orbit, you get a different perspective. And what I've really tried to do in my work, in my thinking, and this book is get outside of ourselves and look at humans in perspective, in the whole sweep of life, and the history of life on Earth, and the whole family of living things that are here with us. Because there's a lot at stake. And not only is there a need, I think, to show what is at stake as we take more and more from the rest of the world, but I wanted to show also who is at stake. Yes. AUDIENCE: Do you think animals, like a dog sitting next to a windowsill, can daydream and have thoughts unassociated to what they see? CARL SAFINA: Do I think animals have daydreams and thoughts unassociated from what they're actually seeing? Yeah, sometimes they do. And I see that in my own dogs. Sometimes my dogs might be lying around. And then they'll suddenly get up, and they'll run outside to go and check and see if there are any squirrels at the bird feeder. Because they love chasing swirls around. So things occur to them. Sometimes they get up and they come over. If I'm writing, and the dog is napping nicely, they'll sometimes wake up, especially one of them. She'll wake up, she'll come, and she'll just start hitting me with her paw. And the door is open. So what does she want? She's just thought that she wants me to go out with her. She wants to maybe go for a run. She's trying to get me to engage with her, because these things cross her mind. If she could never think about going for a run unless we were going for a run, she wouldn't be coming and bothering me to go for a run. Well, yeah, we'll just leave it at that. I can tell you other stories, but it's more or less the same point. Yes. AUDIENCE: So outside of humans as animals, did you observe that some of these other animals, the same species, would develop different cultures depending on where they were? CARL SAFINA: Some animals do develop different cultures depending on where they were. A couple of the most notable are killer whales and chimpanzees. Chimpanzees make simple tools. But the tools they make are different in different populations of chimpanzees. So there are some chimpanzees that fish for termites by using twigs and pieces of grass. They stick it in the termite hole, and the termites bite it to try to defend the hole. And then they pull it out, and then they eat the termites, right? Some of the chimpanzees that do that live in places where there are really hard nuts, and there are rocks. But they have never figured out that you can use rocks to crack nuts. But there are other chimpanzees that use rocks to crack nuts all the time and actually use rocks on another rock like an anvil to crack nuts. Those ones that have figured that out have an advantage. They get more calories because they have a whole other food source that's available to them. So they learn it from one another. That's the definition of culture. They're not born that way. They learn from one another. But the other population doesn't have it yet. So when you think about humans, you have to try to think of before globalization of any kind, before ships got all around anyplace. Human cultures were often very, very different from one another. The technologies were often very, very different from one another. So you see the inkling of that in chimpanzees with simple tools. And those chimps have stone and anvils. And some of them have wooden clubs and anvils. That's not terribly dissimilar to the most advanced tools that many people had until recently, not that long ago, where there are many cultures where the most advanced tool was a bow and arrow. They had no machine of any kind. They had nothing that had an internal moving part of any kind. They mostly had digging sticks, and they had pounders, and they had spears, and bows, and arrows. So anyway, that's chimpanzees. And with killer whales, the cultural differences are strange. One is they have vocal cultural differences. But the strange part is that different groups have some of the same calls, some calls specific to their group only. Other groups they socialize with do not pick up on the calls of that other group. It helps to define which group is which. And even though they mingle, and even though they hear each other, they don't copy all of those sounds that clearly those ones in the groups learn from the other ones. And some of the groups mingle. And some of the groups get close to each other and never meet. and never mingle, like ethnic groups that don't like each other for some reason. And it's not explained, but it's a weird thing. And the closest thing to what those killer whales do is humans, where there are different tribes that won't mix even though we're all exactly the same, except for small cultural or language differences. It's mostly where you're born is what it is. And with those killer whales, it's mostly where they're born, what pod they're born into. And they carry that identity, and they won't mix with others. And it's very odd. Any other questions? Richard. AUDIENCE: Yeah. By the way, that's similar to certain jungle birds. And they'll go into these big, mixed flocks. And they will pay attention to warning cries of predators from another species or another group. And but then they will segregate out again, so it's very similar. The question I want to ask you is this. You say you kept wolves. I didn't know that you kept wolves. CARL SAFINA: What? AUDIENCE: Wolves. CARL SAFINA: I didn't keep wolves myself. No, no, I have dogs. AUDIENCE: Oh, OK. Well OK, I have dogs. And one thing that I've read recently is that one of the innate differences-- and I'm asking you if you know whether this is true or not-- between dogs and wolves is that dogs will follow a human's gaze looking at something. CARL SAFINA: Yes. And right, and you're going to say, and wolves don't, right? MALE SPEAKER: And wolves don't. CARL SAFINA: That's baloney. MALE SPEAKER: OK. CARL SAFINA: And here's why it's baloney. And here's why a lot of the laboratory studies of animals and animal behavior are too simplistic to capture what's there. Dogs were tested to follow pointing with their owners, the people they knew the best. So you take your dog into a lab. And they say, OK, you stand there, and put the dog there. OK, sit. OK, ready, go. Now, point. OK, so the owner points. The dog looks. Then they take wolves with humans that they don't know it all. They put them behind a chain link fence because, oh, my god, they're wolves. And they point. And a wolf behind a chain link fence surrounded by strangers doesn't look where the other person is pointing. But it turns out that when you take hand raised wolves with familiar people, without a chain link fence between them, and you point, the wolves perform as well or better than the dogs with no training. Because they are incredibly keyed into one another in their social groups. When a wolf goes like that, all the other wolves have to see what's interesting over there. Are we about to hunt? Did they just see something? What's going on? AUDIENCE: Well, the claim was that that they did not do that. CARL SAFINA: The claim is disproven. MALE SPEAKER: Good. CARL SAFINA: Put a big X on that claim. One more question, yes. AUDIENCE: It's actually related to that. You're talking-- there's blurb about your book where you describe these cooperative hunting behaviors, like whales knocking a seal off an ice floe and lions attacking in wings. How does that communication work? Do we know about how they coordinate those kind of things? CARL SAFINA: In some cases, we know. And I would say in most cases we pretty much know. And that is, they're very, very tuned into one another. So you mentioned the killer whales that wash the seals off the ice. What was the other thing that you mentioned? AUDIENCE: The lions. CARL SAFINA: The lions, right. So I actually saw some of the thing with the lions. And I didn't see the behavior develop because they knew exactly what they were doing. But I saw them do it. So what I saw was lions are sleeping. We're watching these lions. We were watching those particular lions because that particular pride had a habit of hunting during daylight, which is unusual. So we went to watch and see if they were going to do something really cool. And they were sleeping, and then they woke up. And when they woke up, they got up and they did a big rally with each other, a big greeting. They're all licking each other's faces, and they're all smushing their faces together and everything. And then they're going to go hunt. Now, first of all, you don't need to lick somebody's face in order to go hunting, right? But they do that. Why do they do that? Because that's a reaffirmation. It's just like we wake up and we say good morning to everybody in our family. It's a reaffirmation. We're here. We know who we are. We're together. This is our group identity. So that thing, reestablished in the course of their day, they then leave the area where they were all lying around sleeping. And they leave together, because that greeting helped coordinate them. OK, we're all up now? OK, let's go. They go to a hill. Obviously, they know exactly what hill they're going to because they just went there. They just go to the hill. And then they walk along the crest of the hill. One of them sits down, and the others keep going. And why did that lion just sit down and the other keep going? A second lion sits down, and the rest keep going. A third lion sits. A fourth lion sits, until there is a picket fence of yellow lions in the yellow grass at the crest of this hill. And one lion walks toward the zebras that are about a quarter mile away. And the plan is-- which they've obviously done before and they knew exactly what they were doing-- we'll all wait here. You go chase the zebras up the hill. The zebras were no fools. They detected the lion that was coming, and they spooked and went farther away. So what I saw was really, really interesting, It wasn't incredibly dramatic because the hunt really didn't engage. As far as the killer whales, they fan out, and they're looking in the pack ice, those ones that wash the seals off. They fan out, and they're looking around the pack ice. They can hear each other a long distance underwater, and they call a lot. They recognize each other's voices, like you recognize people on the telephone. They know who's calling underwater. And they're looking mostly for Weddell seals. It's a certain kind of seal that they particularly like. So if they're not food stressed, they're looking for Weddell seals. One of them finds a Weddell seal. It doesn't immediately try to attack the Weddell seal. It backs off and calls until the rest of the group which might, in that case, be three or four individuals, they all come together. They swim away from the ice. And then on some signal, either vocal or visual behavioral, it's OK, now, and then they all go. And they all pump their tails like this in unison, and that creates a big moving wave right above the tails. And they go right to the ice, and then they just dive under the ice. That wave breaks over the ice. It washes the seal off, and then they get the seal that way. And killer whales, more than any other animal in the world, they share their kills. They share a really high percentage of their kills. So it's a really, really cooperative hunting thing. I mean, wolves share the kills too but in a more passive way. The thing is down, and it's just there, and they all eat from it. But the whales actually pull it apart and bring pieces and give to one another. Because they're in a more fluid environment like that. So I'm happy to stay a few more minutes. I don't want to keep you too much longer, because we did start late. And I want to thank you very, very much for coming out. Obviously, we've got some books in the back. Thank you. [APPLAUSE]
B1 中級 卡爾-薩菲娜:"超越語言。動物的思維和感覺"|谷歌講座 (Carl Safina: "Beyond Words: What Animals Think and Feel" | Talks at Google) 811 18 richardwang 發佈於 2021 年 01 月 14 日 更多分享 分享 收藏 回報 影片單字