Placeholder Image

字幕列表 影片播放

  • Professor Paul Bloom: So, most of what we do these

  • daysour methods, our theories,

  • our ideasare shaped, to some extent,

  • by Piaget's influence. And so, what I want to do is

  • begin this class that's going to talk about cognitive development

  • by talking about his ideas. His idea was that children are

  • active thinkers; they're trying to figure out

  • the world. He often described them as

  • little scientists. And incidentally,

  • to know where he's coming from on this, he had a very dramatic

  • and ambitious goal. He didn't start off because he

  • was interested in children. He started off because he was

  • interested in the emergence of knowledge in general.

  • It was a discipline he described as genetic

  • epistemologythe origins of knowledge.

  • But he studied development of the individual child because he

  • was convinced that this development will tell him about

  • the development of knowledge more generally.

  • There's a very snooty phrase that--I don't know if you ever

  • heard it before. It's a great phrase.

  • It's "Ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny."

  • And the idea of this--What that means is that development of an

  • individual mimics or repeats development of the species.

  • Now, it's entirely not true, but it's a beautiful phrase and

  • Piaget was committed to this. He was very interested in

  • saying, "Look. We'll figure how a kid develops

  • and that will tell us about the development of knowledge more

  • generally." So, Piaget viewed the child as

  • a scientist who developed this understanding,

  • these schemas, these little,

  • miniature theories of the world.

  • And they did this through two sorts of mechanisms:

  • assimilation and accommodation. So, assimilation would be the

  • act of expanding the range of things that you respond to.

  • Piaget's example would be a baby who's used to sucking on a

  • breast might come to suck on a bottle or on a rattle.

  • That's changing the scope of things that you respond to.

  • Accommodation is changing how you do it.

  • A baby will form his mouth differently depending on what

  • he's sucking on. And so, these processes where

  • you take in--I'm giving this in a very physical way,

  • but in a more psychological sense you have a way of looking

  • at the world. You could expand it to

  • encompass new things, assimilation.

  • But you could also change your system of knowledge itself

  • accommodation. And Piaget argued that these

  • two mechanisms of learning drove the child through different

  • stages. And he had a stage theory,

  • which was quite different from the Freudian stage theory that

  • we have been introduced to. So his methods were to ask

  • children to solve problems and to ask them questions.

  • And his discoveries that--they did them in different ways at

  • different ages led to the emergence of the Stage Theory.

  • So, for Piaget, the first stage is the

  • sensorimotor stage or the sensorimotor period.

  • For here the child is purely a physical creature.

  • The child has no understanding in any real way of the external

  • world. There's no understanding of the

  • past, no understanding of the future, no stability,

  • no differentiation. The child just touches and

  • sees, but doesn't yet reason.

  • And it's through this stage that a child gradually comes to

  • acquire object permanence.

  • Object permanence is the understanding that things exist

  • when you no longer see them. So those of you in front,

  • you're looking at me and I go. It occurred to me it'd be a

  • great magic trick if I then appeared in back.

  • But no, I'm just here. That's object permanence.

  • If I went under here and then the people said,

  • "Where the hell did he go? Class is over," that would show

  • a lack of object permanence. So, adults have object

  • permanence. Piaget's very interesting claim

  • is that kids don't. Before six-month-olds,

  • Piaget observed, you take an object the kid

  • likes like a rattle, you hide it,

  • you put it behind something, it's like it's gone.

  • And he claimed the child really thinks it's just gone.

  • Things don't continue to exist when I'm not looking at them

  • anymore. And so he noticed they--they're

  • surprised by peek-a-boo. And Piaget's claim was one

  • reason why they're surprised at peek-a-boo is you go--you look

  • at a kid, the kid's smiling and go,

  • "Oh, peek-a-boo," and you close--and you cover your face

  • and the kid says, "He's gone."

  • "Peek-a-boo." "Oh, there he is.

  • He's gone." And you really--That's the

  • claim. Piaget also discovered that

  • older children fail at a task that's known as the A-not-B

  • task. And Peter Gray in his

  • psychology textbook refers to it as the "changing hiding places"

  • problem, which is probably a better name for it.

  • And here's the idea. You take a nine-month-old and

  • for Piaget a nine-month-old is just starting to make sense of

  • objects and their permanence. You take an object and you put

  • it here in a cup where the kid can't see it,

  • but it's in the cup. So the kid, if you were the

  • kid, will reach for it. You do it again, reach for it.

  • You do it again, reach for it. That's point A.

  • Then you take--you move it over here.

  • Piaget observed kids would still reach for this.

  • It's like they're not smart enough to figure out that it's

  • not there anymore, even if they see it move.

  • And this was more evidence that they just don't understand

  • objects, and that this thing takes a lot of time and learning

  • to develop. The next stage is the

  • preoperational stage. The child starts off grasping

  • the world only in a physical way, in a sensorimotor way,

  • but when he gets to the preoperational period the

  • capacity to represent the world, to have the world inside your

  • head, comes into being. But it's limited and it's

  • limited in a couple of striking ways.

  • One way in which it's limited is that children are egocentric.

  • Now, egocentrism has a meaning in common English which means to

  • be selfish. Piaget meant it in a more

  • technical way. He claimed that children at

  • this age literally can't understand that others can see

  • the world differently from them. So, one of his demonstrations

  • was the three mountains task. We have three mountains over

  • there. You put a child on one side of

  • the mountains and you ask him to draw it, and a four- or

  • five-year-old can do it easily, but then you ask him to draw it

  • as it would appear from the other side and children find

  • this extraordinarily difficult. They find it very difficult to

  • grasp the world as another person might see it.

  • Another significant finding Piaget had about this phase of

  • development concerns what's called "conservation."

  • The notion of conservation is that there's ways to transform

  • things such that some aspects of them change but others remain

  • the same. So, for instance,

  • if you take a glass of water and you pour it into another

  • glass that's shallow or tall, it won't change the amount of

  • water you have. If you take a bunch of pennies

  • and you spread them out, you don't get more pennies.

  • But kids, according to Piaget, don't know that and this is one

  • of the real cool demonstrations. Any of you who have access to a

  • four- or five-year-old, [laughter]

  • a sibling or something--Do not take one without permission,

  • but if you have access to a four- or five-year-old you can

  • do this yourself. This is what it looks like.

  • The first one has no sound. The second one is going to be

  • sound that's going to come on at the end.

  • But there's two rows of checkers.

  • She asks the kid which one has more.

  • The kid says they're the same. Then she says--Now she asks him

  • which one has more, that or that.

  • So that's really stupid. And it's an amazing finding

  • kids will do that and it's a robust finding.

  • Here's another example. So, they're the same.

  • So, it's a cool finding of that stage, suggesting a limitation

  • in how you deal and make sense of the world.

  • The next phase, concrete operations,

  • from seven to twelve, you can solve the conservation

  • problem, but still you're limited to the

  • extent you're capable of abstract reasoning.

  • So the mathematical notions of infinity or logical notions like

  • logical entailment are beyond a child of this age.

  • The child is able to do a lot, but still it's to some extent

  • stuck in the concrete world. And then finally,

  • at around age twelve, you could get abstract and

  • scientific reasoning. And this is the Piagetian

  • theory in very brief form. Now, Piaget fared a lot better

  • than did Freud or Skinner for several reasons.

  • One reason is these are interesting and falsifiable

  • claims about child development. So claims that--about the

  • failure of conservation in children at different ages could

  • be easily tested and systematically tested,

  • and in fact, there's a lot of support for

  • them. Piaget had a rich theoretical

  • framework, pulling together all sorts of observations in

  • different ways, wrote many, many books and

  • articles and articulated his theory very richly.

  • And most of all, I think, he had some really

  • striking findings. Before Piaget,

  • nobody noticed these conservation findings.

  • Before Piaget, nobody noticed that babies had

  • this problem tracking and understanding objects.

  • At the same time, however, there are limitations

  • in Piaget's theory. Some of these limitations are

  • theoretical. It's an interesting question as

  • to whether he really explains how a child goes from a concrete

  • thinker to an abstract thinker, or how he goes from not having

  • object permanence to understanding object permanence.

  • There's methodological limitations.

  • Piaget was really big into question and answer,

  • but one problem with this is that children aren't very good

  • with language, and this might lead you to

  • underestimate how much they know.

  • And this is particularly a problem the younger you get.

  • Methodology is going to loom heavy in the discussion of any

  • science and that includes psychology.

  • Often 90% of the game is discovering a clever method

  • through which to test your hypotheses.

  • We're going to talk a little bit about that regarding babies.

  • I'll give you another example from a very different domain.

  • There was a set of scientists interested in studying tickling.

  • So, when you tickle somebody, under what circumstances will

  • they laugh? Where do you have to tickle

  • them? Can you tickle yourself?

  • Does it have to be a surprise, and so on?

  • It turns out very difficult to study this in a lab.

  • You're not going to have your experimental credit.

  • You come into the lab and say, "Okay.

  • I'm the graduate student. Ha, ha, ha."

  • And [laughter] in fact, an example of a

  • methodological attempt was done by Henry Gleitman at University

  • of Pennsylvania, who built a tickle machine,

  • which was this box with these two giant hands that went

  • "r-r-r-r." This was a failure because

  • people could not go near the tickle machine without

  • convulsing in laughter. But we will discuss when we

  • have a lecture on laughter a bit of the tickle sciences.

  • And finally there's factual. What do infants and children

  • really know? It's possible that due to the

  • methodological limitations of Piaget, he systematically

  • underestimated what children and babies know.

  • And in fact, I'll present some evidence

  • suggesting that this is in fact--that this is the case.

  • So, I want to introduce you to the modern science of infant

  • cognition. Infant cognition has been

  • something studied for a very long time.

  • And there was a certain view that has had behind it a

  • tremendous philosophical and psychological consensus.

  • And it's summarized in this Onion headline here.

  • And the idea is that babies are stupid, that babies really don't

  • know much about the world. Now, the work that this

  • Onion headline is satirizing is the recent

  • studies, which I'm going to talk about,

  • suggested that on the contrary, babies might be smarter than

  • you think. And to discover the

  • intelligence of babies we have to ourselves be pretty smart in

  • developing different techniques. To study what a baby knows,

  • you can't ask your questions. Babies can't talk.

  • You could look at what it does but babies are not very

  • coordinated or skilled so you need to use clever methods.

  • One clever method is to look at their brain waves [laughter].

  • This child on the right died during testing.

  • It was a tragic--It was crushed by the weights [laughter]

  • of the electrodes. He's happy though.

  • You could study their brain waves.

  • One of the few things babies can do is they could suck on a

  • pacifier. And you might think,

  • well, how could you learn anything from that?

  • Well, for instance, you could build machines that

  • when babies suck on a pacifier they hear music or they hear

  • language, and then you could look at how

  • much they suck on the pacifier to determine what they like.

  • But undeniably we know most of our--we got most of our

  • knowledge about babies from studies of their looking times.

  • That's one thing babies can do. They can look.

  • And I have up here--This is a picture of Elizabeth Spelke,

  • who is a developmental psychologist who's developed the

  • most research on looking at babies' looking times and what

  • you could learn from them. And I have here two ways you

  • could learn from looking. One is preference.

  • So for instance, suppose you want to know,

  • for whatever reason, do babies like the looks of

  • dogs or cats? Well, you could put a baby

  • down, have a picture of a dog here, a picture of a cat here,

  • and see which one the baby looks at.

  • Babies can move their eyes and that could tell you something.

  • Do babies distinguish pretty faces from ugly faces?

  • Well, put a pretty face here, an ugly face here,

  • see if the baby prefers to look at the pretty one.

  • You could also do habituation and surprise.

  • And much of the studies I'm going to talk about here involve

  • habituation and surprise. Habituation is a fancy word for

  • boredom. What you do is you show a baby

  • something over and over again. Now, remember from behaviorism

  • the baby will learn this isn't very interesting.

  • Then you show the baby something different.

  • If the baby really sees it as different, the baby will look

  • longer, and you could use that as a measure of what babies find

  • different. For instance,

  • suppose you want to know if the baby can tell green from red.

  • Well, you could show the baby a green patch, a green patch,

  • a green patch, a green patch;

  • the baby'll get bored, then a red patch.

  • If they all look the same to the baby, the baby will just

  • continue to tune out, but if the red looks different

  • the baby will perk up. And this is,

  • in fact, one way they study color vision in babies.

  • Surprise is related to this. You could show babies something

  • that shouldn't happen. If babies are like--If babies

  • also think it shouldn't happen, they might look longer,

  • and essentially what happens is scientists do magic tricks to

  • explore this very thing. And to start with some real

  • examples, a lot of this infant research has gone back to the

  • Piagetian question of object permanence,

  • asking, "Is it really true babies don't know that objects

  • remain even when they're out of sight?"

  • So one very simple study by Spelke and Baillargeon:

  • Have babies shown a block with a bar going back and forth like

  • that. So the bar just goes back and

  • forth. Now, there's something you do

  • that's so obvious you probably don't even know you're doing it.

  • When you see a display like that, what you assume is there's

  • a bar there, and what that means is there's something in the

  • middle that you've never seen before.

  • But of course, if you were a simple perceptual

  • creature, you would just see that there'd be a bar on top and

  • a bar on the bottom. You wouldn't expect anything in

  • the middle because you never saw anything in the middle.

  • So, what you do then is you show babies this and then you

  • show them either B or C and if we do this with adults you

  • expect B, C is almost a joke. And, in fact,

  • babies respond the same way. Babies expect there to be an

  • entire, complete bar and are surprised and look longer at the

  • broken bar. Other studies,

  • some of them--Well, here's another study by Rene

  • Baillargeon looking at the same thing in a different way.

  • You show the baby, say a six-month-old,

  • a stage with a block on it. Then a screen rises and

  • obscures the block. Now, if the babies expect the

  • block to still be there, they should think the block

  • should stop the screen. On the other hand,

  • if out of sight out of mind, they should expect the screen

  • to keep going. So, what you do is you set up a

  • couple of displays, one where the block is stopped,

  • the other one where you take this away with a trap door and

  • it keeps going. And, as you see,

  • the baby screams when this happens.

  • That doesn't really happen, but they do look longer.

  • One final example of an object permanence study.

  • Some of this work's been done at Yale in Karen Wynn's lab,

  • where they look at babies' understanding of addition and

  • subtraction. And a lot of it is done with

  • real objects, but there's also animated

  • versions so here is an animated example.

  • Babies are surprised. They expect 2 - 1 = 1 and when

  • 2 - 1 = 2 or 3 or 0, they look longer,

  • indicating surprise. And even six-month-olds are

  • sensitive to these rudimentary facts of arithmetic,

  • telling us something about their mathematical knowledge,

  • but also telling us something about that they expect things to

  • remain when they're out of sight.

  • Now, this research suggests that infants' understanding of

  • the physical world is there from the very start,

  • but at the same time not entirely.

  • We know there are certain things babies don't know.

  • Here's an example. Suppose you show babies this.

  • You have a block here and then you have something above there

  • floating in mid air. Babies find this surprising.

  • Even six-month-olds find this surprising.

  • It violates gravity, but six-month-olds aren't smart

  • enough to know that a block just stuck over here is also

  • surprising. Twelve-month-olds will think

  • that it should fall. Six-month-olds don't,

  • and even 12-month-olds don't find anything weird about this,

  • while adults are sophisticated enough to understand that that's

  • an unstable configuration and should fall over.

  • So, although some things are built in, some things develop.

  • And this raises the question of, "How do we explain

  • development?" How do we explain when babies

  • come to know things that they didn't originally know?

  • Well, one answer is neural maturation, growth of the brain.

  • Most of the neurons you have now in your head,

  • right now, you had when you were in your mother's uterus.

  • What happens in development isn't for the most part the

  • growth of new neurons. It's for the most part pruning,

  • getting rid of neurons. So, the neural structures

  • change radically as babies kind of get rid of excess neurons

  • through development. At the same time though,

  • connections between neurons grow like crazy and they--and

  • this process of synaptic growth where there are the connections

  • across different synapses peaks at about two years.

  • Finally, remember myelination, where you sort of get this

  • fatty sheath over your neuron to make it more effective?

  • That also happens through development, and in fact,

  • it goes through development and even teenagers are not fully

  • myelinated. In particular,

  • they're not fully myelinated in their frontal lobes.

  • Recall that frontal lobes are involved in things like

  • restraint and willpower. And so, it could be the problem

  • is the baby's brain doesn't develop yet.

  • Another possibility is there's problems with inhibition.

  • This is related, again, to the frontal lobes and

  • this comes out with the A, not B error.

  • So, remember the baby reaches, reaches, reaches.

  • It's moved, reach, follow, keeps reaching the same

  • place. And it could be that babies

  • don't know anything about objects.

  • But another possibility is once you do something it's kind of

  • hard to stop. It takes a bit of control to

  • stop. And there's all sorts of

  • independent evidence that babies lack this control.

  • The part of their brain that could control certain behaviors

  • is just not active yet. There's a very nice

  • illustration of inhibitory problems from a "Simpsons"

  • episode that actually sort of covers anything you might want

  • to know about developmental differences.

  • And that basically may sum up much of developmental

  • psychology. That the child essentially--he

  • does A, A, A. It's moved.

  • You go, "doh!" and he keeps going for it.

  • And there's some evidence that's true.

  • Adele Diamond who studies this finds that although kids reach

  • for A, they look for B, as if they know it's there but

  • they can't stop themselves from reaching.

  • And we'll continue this theme a little bit later.

  • Finally, it might be kids don't know things.

  • Some things you've got to learn. And this is true in all sorts

  • of domainsin the social world, in the economic world,

  • in the political worldand it's true as well in the

  • physical world. In fact, there's some things

  • even adults don't know. So, here's a study by Michael

  • McCloskey with college students. Here's the idea.

  • You have a tube, a transparent--a tube--a hollow

  • tube, and at the top of the tube you throw a ball through so it

  • whips through the tube and it comes out.

  • The question is, "What happens to it?"

  • Does it go in the path of A, or does it go in the path of B?

  • Without looking around, who votes for A?

  • Who votes for B? Here's the weird thing.

  • Whenever I do this at Yale everybody gets the damn thing

  • right [laughter]. At Johns Hopkins,

  • 50/50, [laughter] for A and B.

  • I got to get a better demo. But anyway, college students

  • not here, show systematic biases of incorrect physical

  • intuitions. Here's a twist,

  • and if you found people who were less wonderful than you

  • all, and asked them you'd get a lot of people saying the curving

  • thing. But here's a twist.

  • Ask somebody, "What if you took a tube and

  • you squirted water through it? Where would the water go?"

  • Nobody chooses B. Everybody knows the water would

  • continue in a straight line, suggesting that when you have

  • experience that helps you out, but in absence of experience

  • you're kind of lost. We've talked about the physical

  • world. What about the social world?

  • What about the world of people? Well, there's a lot of research

  • on this as well. Babies start off with some

  • social preferences. If you take newborn

  • babies--It's very hard to do research with newborn babies

  • actually because of the consent procedure and everything,

  • so most of this work is done in France [laughter],

  • where they have no laws at all. They just rush in to--Women

  • give birth and they rush in and they say, "We are

  • psychologists," and then we do experiments on

  • the babies, and it's terrific. And this is one of them where

  • they compare babies looking at this versus this.

  • Babies like the one that looks like a face.

  • These are newborns. There are some preferences with

  • humans and with other primates to favor faces.

  • Babies are also social animals too, so they're natural mimics.

  • Andrew Meltzoff, for instance,

  • has found that if you go to a newborn baby,

  • and if you find a newborn baby, this is the first thing you

  • should do. Stick your face right up to the

  • newborn baby and go like this and stick your tongue out.

  • And Meltzoff finds that babies more often than not stick their

  • tongues out back, suggesting some sort of social

  • connection from one person to another, and then later on

  • babies are mimics. Babies more often than not will

  • copy the face next to them. Now, these--the nature of these

  • responses, this preferring faces, this sort of mimicry,

  • is a matter of debate, and there's a lot of research

  • going on asking how smart are babies.

  • Can we see--use some of the same methods that we've looked

  • at for the physical world to look at the social world?

  • And to illustrate one of the studies, I'll tell you about a

  • study that I did with Valerie Kuhlmeier and Karen Wynn.

  • And so, what we tested was nine-month-olds and

  • twelve-month-olds, and we showed them movies.

  • So, they're sitting down and they're seeing a movie where one

  • character's going to help a ball achieve a goal,

  • and another character's going to hinder the ball.

  • And then we're going to see whether they expect the ball to

  • approach the one that helped it versus the one that hindered it.

  • So, this is what a baby would see.

  • This is literally the same movie a baby would see in the

  • experiment. The thing is for these sorts of

  • experiments there is a lot of control, so something that's a

  • square in one movie will be a triangle in another movie;

  • something that's on the top in one movie will be on the bottom

  • in another movie. So, this is an example movie

  • but this is what babies would see.

  • And they'd see this over and over again and the question is

  • would they expect babies--would babies expect the one to

  • approach the one that helped it or approach the one that

  • hindered it? And what we find is,

  • statistically, babies look longer when shown a

  • movie where it approaches the one that hindered it versus

  • helped it. And this we take as preliminary

  • evidence that they have a social interpretation.

  • They see this movie as you see this movie in terms of helping

  • and hindering, and somebody going to somebody

  • that helped it versus hindered it.

  • You could then ask--This makes a prediction that babies should

  • themselves prefer the creature who's the helper versus the

  • hinderer, and to explore this,

  • a graduate student in this department, Kiley Hamlin,

  • has started a series of studies where they show babies

  • three-dimensional scenes and then give them the characters

  • and see which one they reach for.

  • So, here's video so you could see how this experiment is done.

  • Now, the next trial is from a different study.

  • A different thing we use, and the baby is given a choice.

  • One thing to know methodologically is the person

  • giving a choice is blind to the study.

  • And blind here is a technical term meaning she had no idea

  • what the baby saw, and the point about this is to

  • avoid either intentional or unintentional sort of trying to

  • get the answer you want. She couldn't do that because

  • she didn't know what the right answer is.

  • So, here's what the baby would see.

  • So, this suggests that some social understanding may be

  • there from the very start. This evidence is tentative,

  • very controversial. But now, I want to raise a huge

  • developmental puzzle and the puzzle is there are some ways in

  • which babies are--not just babies,

  • but young children are very clueless when it comes to

  • people. And so, I have a film clip here

  • of two very nice studies showing babies' ignorant--sorry,

  • young children's ignorance of other people.

  • I'll show you the studies and then we'll briefly discuss what

  • they mean. Professor Paul Bloom:

  • Before discussing that example in a little bit more detail,

  • any questions? What are your questions?

  • Yes, in back. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: Typically--I don't know for

  • those particular children, but typically on those tasks

  • three-year-olds and young four-year-olds tend to fail,

  • and around the age of four or five kids tend to succeed.

  • There's sort of a period around the age of four,

  • four and a half, where kids make the transition

  • from failure to success. The question,

  • by the way, was when do children--in that video when

  • were the--what were the ages of the children who failed and who

  • passed?

  • Yes. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: The question of whether

  • discriminant conditioning has been used with babies to explore

  • what sort of concepts they have. I don't know.

  • Does anybody--It has been-- Graduate Student:

  • --It's not as effective-- Professor Paul Bloom:

  • Koleen answered and said that it's not as effective as other

  • methods. Part of the problem with using

  • operant conditioning with babies is it's difficult to get them to

  • behave in any systematic way. So, the looking-time measures

  • tend to be more subtle. Any other questions?

  • Oh. Yes.

  • Student: [inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:

  • Oh. The question of why they

  • chose--the baby--the kids chose the rocket ship one as opposed

  • to the Rafael one. It wasn't what they were

  • interested in in the experiment. And my bet is when they chose

  • the stickers they had a pretty good sense of why,

  • of which ones the boys would prefer in those studies.

  • The question of why a boy might prefer one sort of sticker,

  • and you might get a different response with a girl,

  • is going to come up later when we discuss different theories of

  • sex differences. But that was something I think

  • they were just assuming in the study to get it off the ground.

  • Okay.

  • There's a huge debate over what's going on there.

  • And if you listened at the end to the psychologist summarizing

  • the data, the psychologist had a very good and very clear and

  • strong idea of what was going on.

  • It was that children need to know more about minds.

  • The children don't know about that you can do something with

  • the intent to deceive. They don't understand that

  • somebody could choose what you chose in a malicious way.

  • This is possible. This is one respectable theory,

  • but the alternative is they have the right knowledge,

  • but they suffer from problems with inhibition.

  • So, consider both studies. The first study,

  • the one with the deceptive dolls with the big shoes and

  • little shoes, is actually fairly difficult.

  • And it's possible that children kind of got overwhelmed with it,

  • and when asked what would the mother think,

  • who the mother would think stole the food,

  • responded with who really stole the food.

  • And that there's some pull towards the right answer that

  • makes this task difficult. The second one--the second

  • study illustrates this issue even more clearly.

  • Take the boy who kept failing. He kept pointing to the rocket

  • ship and mean monkey kept taking it away.

  • It's possible that he genuinely didn't know what to do,

  • that he wasn't smart enough to understand that he needed to

  • point to the other one. But it's also possible that

  • it's a Homer Simpson-like effect, where when asked to

  • point to what he wants, he just couldn't help but point

  • to the one he wanted. And that the extra work it

  • takes to lie was beyond him. And, in support of the second

  • alternative, even adults find these tasks involving lying and

  • deception more difficult. They were slower at them.

  • We make more mistakes than tasks that don't involve lying

  • and deception. So, I'm raising this not to

  • solve the problem. You'll read more about it in

  • the Peter Gray textbook and more about it in The Norton

  • readings on development, but just to raise this as an

  • interesting area of debate. Another interesting area of

  • debate is, "What's the relationship between different

  • sorts of development?" So, I started off with Piaget,

  • and Piaget, like Freud, believed in general,

  • across the board changes in how children think.

  • An alternative, though, is that there's

  • separate modules, and this is a view developed,

  • again, by Noam Chomsky, and also by the philosopher of

  • mind Jerry Fodor, who claimed that the whole idea

  • of a child developing as a single story is mistaken.

  • What you get instead is there are separate pre-wired systems

  • for reasoning about the world. These systems have some

  • built-in knowledge, and they have to do some

  • learning, but the learning pattern varies

  • from system to system and there's a separateness to them.

  • Why should we take this view seriously?

  • Well, one reason is that there are developmental disorders that

  • seem to involve damage to one system but not to another.

  • And the classic case of this is a disorder known as autism.

  • And autism is something I've always found a fascinating

  • disorder for many reasons. It's actually why I entered

  • psychology. I started off working with

  • children with autism. And it could be taken as a

  • striking illustration of how the social part of your brain is

  • distinct from other parts of your brain.

  • So, what autism is is a disorder that strikes about one

  • in a thousand people, mostly boys.

  • And the dominant problems concern--consist of a lack of

  • social connectedness, problems with language,

  • problems dealing with people, and more generally,

  • a problem of what the psychologist,

  • Simon Baron-Cohen has described as "mind blindness."

  • In that autistic people show no impairments dealing with the

  • physical world, they show no impairments

  • on--they don't necessarily show any impairments on mathematical

  • skills or spatial skills, but they have a lot of problems

  • with people. Now, many autistic children

  • have no language; they're totally shut off from

  • society. But even some of them who'd

  • learned language and who managed to get some sort of independent

  • life, nevertheless will suffer from a

  • severe social impairment. And this could be shown in all

  • sorts of ways. A simple experiment developed

  • by Simon Baron-Cohen goes like this.

  • You show this to three- and four-year-olds.

  • There's four candies there, and you say,

  • "This is Charlie in the middle. Which chocolate will Charlie

  • take?" For most children and most of

  • you, I hope, the answer's pretty clear: This one.

  • Autistic children will often just shrug, say,

  • "How could I know?" because they don't

  • instinctively appreciate that people's interests and desires

  • tend to be attuned to where they're looking.

  • Another sort of task, which is a task that's been

  • done hundreds, perhaps thousands of times,

  • is known as "the false-belief task" and here's the idea.

  • You show the child the following situation.

  • There's a doll named Maxie and Maxie puts the ball in the

  • cupboard. Maxie leaves and a second doll

  • enters. The second doll takes the ball

  • out of the cupboard and puts it under the bed.

  • Maxie comes back and the question is, "Where will Maxie

  • look for the ball?" Now, this is a question about

  • your understanding about minds. The question of where is the

  • ball really is a question about the physical world.

  • Everyone can solve it, but this question is hard.

  • The right answer is Max will--Maxie will look in the

  • cupboard, even though it's not really there because Maxie has a

  • false belief about the world. Three-year-olds find this

  • difficult. Two-year-olds find this

  • difficult. Four-year-olds and

  • five-year-olds are able to pass this task.

  • Normal adults are able to pass this task.

  • Children with autism have serious problems.

  • And often, people with autism who are otherwise very high

  • functioning will fail this task. They'll say,

  • "Oh, he must think it's not--He'll--He's going to check

  • under the bed." Any questions about autism?

  • Yes. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: Good question.

  • It isn't. They're both experiments

  • designed to tap an appreciation of false belief.

  • The deception one with the shoes and everything looked at

  • it in the course of deception. Can you understand that the

  • mother might think it's that person even though it's really

  • that person? And our kid failed.

  • This is a sort of stripped-down version without all the

  • fanciness but it tests exactly the same thing.

  • Yes. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: Nobody knows,

  • but there's a theory which won't answer your question but

  • will put it into a broader context.

  • Simon Baron-Cohen argues that there are certain abilities that

  • tend to be more sequestered for males,

  • and other abilities that are more sequestered,

  • more focused on females. Social abilities,

  • he argues, tend to be more female than male.

  • So, the way Baron puts it, provocatively,

  • is to be a man is to suffer from a very mild form of autism

  • [laughter]. The idea is then that autistic

  • individuals suffer from what he calls extreme male brains,

  • and as such, it stands to reason that they'd

  • be more sampled from the male population than the female

  • population. That's such an interesting

  • issue, that again, when we return to talk about

  • sex differences we'll look at that in a little bit more detail

  • to see if it's supported by the evidence.

  • Yes. Student: [inaudible]

  • Professor Paul Bloom: I'm sorry.

  • Tell me the--Is the severity of autismStudent:

  • [inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:

  • It's an interesting question. The question is,

  • "How do you think about the severity of autism with regard

  • to developmental stages?" And sort of surprisingly,

  • autism can't really be thought of in that way.

  • So, it's not like an adult with autism is like a three-year-old

  • or a two-year-old. In some ways,

  • somebody with autism isn't like any child at all,

  • any normally developing child at all.

  • So, it's not really a developmental delay in the way

  • that it might make sense to think about certain forms of

  • retardation. On the other hand,

  • when we think about how severe autism is we do look at things

  • like how much language does the person have,

  • and in that sense, it is related to development.

  • Yes. Student:

  • What are the chances that someone who's autistic would be

  • able to overcome their deficiencies?

  • Professor Paul Bloom: The majority of people with

  • autism. It's a good question.

  • The question is, "What are the chances that

  • somebody with autism will be able to overcome their

  • deficiencies?" Autism is a funny disorder in

  • that there's a lot of media publication and media

  • presentation. Often the people who are

  • showcased in the media tend to be very exceptional.

  • So, there's a woman, Temple Grandin,

  • who's autistic and--Has anybody here heard of Temple Grandin?

  • She wrote some wonderful books about her experience as an

  • autistic person, but she's very unusual.

  • So a lot depends, to answer your question,

  • how one defines autism, and whether one includes

  • Asperger syndrome, which is a limited,

  • a more mild syndrome, as a form of autism.

  • The answer is that the majority of people with autism have

  • severe problems, and will not,

  • and at this stage, with this level of therapy,

  • will not lead a normal life. Student:

  • More specifically, what I meant was,

  • when you showed the example of Rain Man, ere they exceptional

  • [inaudible] Professor Paul Bloom:

  • Right. The question is about so-called

  • autistic savants. So, Rain Man,

  • the character played by Dustin Hoffman, had extraordinary

  • mathematical abilities. And some people with autism

  • have extraordinary artistic abilities or mathematical

  • abilities or musical abilities and these are amazing.

  • It's an amazing question why they have it but this is a very

  • small minority. This is a very--It's

  • fascinating that it happens at all, that you have severe damage

  • but compensated with some powerful skill.

  • Now, I know I'm answering your question I think in a better

  • way, but it's actually very rare.

  • Most people with autism do not have any exceptional abilities

  • that go along with it.

  • Another question is if you believe in modules--If there are

  • modules, what are they? And so far when reviewing the

  • developmental data we've talked about two of them:

  • physics and people. An object module and a social

  • module. But other people have argued

  • that there is a special module in your brain for dealing with

  • artifacts, that is, things like tables and

  • chairs and cars and forks. Some people have argued there's

  • a module for sociology, for dealing with human groups,

  • races and classes and so on. Some have even argued that

  • there is an intuitive biology, a common-sense biological

  • understanding of the world that's separate from your

  • understanding of people and physics.

  • And, in fact, the most dominant proponent of

  • the view is our very own Frank Keil,

  • Master of Morse College at Yale, who has strongly defended

  • the notion of an intuitive biological module.

  • Final question, just to raise:

  • I've talked in terms of the modular view but there might

  • also be profound general differences between children and

  • adults, not just specific to how you

  • think about objects or how you think about people or how you

  • think about this or how you think about that,

  • but rather more general. And one claim,

  • which we're going to return to briefly next class when we talk

  • about language, is that there's a very,

  • very big difference between a creature that doesn't have

  • language and a creature that does.

  • And part of the claim is that learning a language,

  • learning to speak, reconfigures the human brain in

  • such a way that is really exceptional.

  • And that has no parallel in any other species.

  • And this is an interesting claim and one we'll talk about.

  • Finally, I want to end with an example from Stephen Jay Gould.

  • Suppose you hate development; you hate developmental

  • psychology; you hate babies;

  • you hate children; they're not cute;

  • they're ugly; you don't want to have them;

  • you don't want to study them; you're annoyed that we have to

  • discuss them. Fine.

  • But there are reasons to study development even if you are not

  • interested in children because sometimes developmental studies

  • and developmental data and developmental science can inform

  • questions about adults. And Stephen Jay Gould has a

  • very nice example of this. He asked the question "Is a

  • zebra a black animal with white stripes or a white animal with

  • black stripes?" Now, you could look at adult

  • zebras all day long and you're never going to figure this out.

  • But if you want to know the answer, and I knew it,

  • but I forget what it is--It doesn't matter.

  • But if you wanted to know it you could.

  • You would look at development and you'd watch the

  • embryological development of a zebra and that's how you would

  • learn the answer to your question.

  • In fact, I'll end with a nice quote.

  • This is by the famous biologist, D'Arcy Thompson,

  • who wrote the book On Growth and Form,

  • and it's sort of the model of many developmental

  • psychologists and many evolutionary psychologists so

  • I'll end with this: "Everything is the way it is

  • because it got that way." Okay.

  • I'll see you next week.

Professor Paul Bloom: So, most of what we do these

字幕與單字

單字即點即查 點擊單字可以查詢單字解釋

B1 中級

5.寶寶是什麼樣子的。思想的發展 (5. What Is It Like to Be a Baby: The Development of Thought)

  • 132 34
    CH 發佈於 2021 年 01 月 14 日
影片單字