Placeholder Image

字幕列表 影片播放

  • Behavior is action in response to a stimulus.

  • My cat Cameo is now responding to both an external stimulus

  • the sound of a bag of treats, and an internal stimulus

  • her hunger, or at least her insatiable desire for treats.

  • Sometimes animal behavior can seem really far out,

  • but if you look closely enough,

  • you can see how all behavior serves a purpose

  • to help an animal mate, eat, avoid predators, and raise young.

  • And since behaviors can come with advantages like these,

  • natural selection acts on them just as it acts on physical traits

  • ensuring the success of animals who engage

  • in beneficial behaviors, while weeding out those that do stupid,

  • dangerous or otherwise unhelpful stuff.

  • The most beneficial behaviors are those that make an animal

  • better at doing the only two things in the world that matter:

  • eating and sex.

  • Still, that doesn't mean all behavior is about

  • just looking out for number one.

  • It turns out some advantageous behavior

  • is actually pretty selfless.

  • More on that in a minute.

  • But first, behavior is really just a product of a pair of factors:

  • Morphology, or the physical structure of an animal

  • and physiology, or the function of that morphology.

  • Now, an animal's behavior is obviously limited

  • by what its body is capable of doing

  • for example, Cameo does not have opposable thumbs,

  • so, much as she would like to get into the treat bag,

  • by herself, she cannot.

  • This limitation is strictly hereditary

  • no cats can open treat bags with their thumbs

  • because no cats have opposable thumbs.

  • Though some cats do have thumbs.

  • In the same way that a penguin can't fly to escape a predator;

  • or a gazelle can't reach the same leaves as a giraffe can.

  • Similarly, behavior is constrained by an animal's physiology.

  • Like, Cameo's built for chasing down little critters

  • and eating meat, not beds of lettuce.

  • This is because her physiology, everything from her teeth

  • to her digestive system, are geared for eating meat.

  • If she pounced on and ate every blade of grass she came across...

  • let's just say I would not want to be in charge of that litter box.

  • Now the traits that make up an animal's morphology

  • and physiology are often heritable,

  • so we generally talk about selection acting on those traits.

  • But as natural selection hones these traits,

  • it's really selecting their associated behaviors.

  • It's the USE of the trait,

  • using wings and feathers to escape predators,

  • or using a long neck to reach leaves,

  • that provides the evolutionary advantage.

  • Still, that doesn't mean all behavior is coded

  • in an animal's genes

  • some behaviors are learned.

  • And even for animals that learn how to do things,

  • natural selection has favored brain structures

  • that are capable of learning.

  • So one way or another,

  • most behaviors have some genetic underpinning,

  • and we call those behaviors adaptive.

  • Problem is, it's not always obvious

  • what the evolutionary advantages are for some of the nutty things

  • that animals do.

  • Like, why does a snapping turtle always stick out its tongue?

  • How does a tiny Siberian hamster find its mate,

  • miles across the unforgiving tundra?

  • Why does a bower bird collect piles of garbage?

  • To answer questions like those,

  • we have to figure out what stimulus causes these behaviors,

  • and what functions the behaviors serve.

  • To do this, I'm going to need the help of one of the first

  • animal behavior scientists ever, or ethologists, Niko Tinbergen.

  • Tinbergen developed a set of four questions

  • aimed at understanding animal behavior.

  • The questions focus on how a behavior occurs,

  • and why natural selection has favored this particular behavior.

  • Determining how a behavior occurs actually involves two questions:

  • One: what stimulus causes it?

  • And two: what does the animal's body do

  • in response to that stimulus?

  • These are the causes that are closest

  • to the specific behavior we're looking at,

  • so they're called the proximate causes.

  • In the case of the male Siberian hamster,

  • the stimulus is a delicious smelling pheromone

  • that the sexy female hamster releases when she's ready to mate.

  • The male hamster's response, of course, is to scuttle,

  • surprisingly quickly, over several miles if necessary

  • to find and mate with her.

  • So the proximate cause of this behavior

  • was that the girl hamster signaled that she was ready to knock boots,

  • and the male ran like crazy to get to the boot-knockin'.

  • Asking the more complex question of why natural selection

  • has favored this behavior requires asking two more questions:

  • One: what about this behavior helps this animal survive

  • and/or reproduce?

  • And two: what is the evolutionary history of this behavior?

  • These, as you can tell, are bigger-picture questions,

  • and they show us the ultimate causes of the behavior.

  • The answer to the first question, of course,

  • is that the ability of a male hamster to detect and respond

  • to the pheromones of an ovulating female is directly linked

  • to his reproductive success!

  • As for the second question, you can also see that male hamsters

  • with superior pheromone detectors will be able to find females

  • more successfully than other male hamsters,

  • and thereby produce more offspring.

  • So natural selection has honed this particular physical ability

  • and behavior over generations of hamsters.

  • So, who would have thought to ask these questions in the first place?

  • And where's my chair?

  • Niko Tinbergen was one third of a trifecta

  • of revolutionary ethologists in the 20th century.

  • Along with Austrians Karl von Frisch and Konrad Lorenz,

  • he provided a foundation for studying animal behavior

  • and applied these ideas to the study of specific behaviors

  • and for that all three shared the Nobel Prize in 1973.

  • You may have seen the famous photos of young graylag geese

  • following obediently in a line behind a man.

  • That was Lorenz, and his experiments

  • first conducted in the 1930s introduced the world to imprinting,

  • the formation of social bonds in infant animals,

  • and the behavior that includes both learned and innate components.

  • When he observed newly hatched ducklings and geese,

  • he discovered that waterfowl in particular

  • had no innate recognition of their mothers.

  • In the case of graylag geese, he found the imprinting stimulus

  • to be any nearby object moving away from the young!

  • So when incubator-hatched goslings spent

  • their first hours with Lorenz,

  • not only did they follow him,

  • but they showed no recognition of their real mother

  • or other adults in their species!

  • Unfortunately, Lorenz was also a member of the Nazi party

  • from 1938 to 1943.

  • And in response to some of his studies

  • on degenerative features that arose in hybrid geese,

  • Lorenz warned that it took only a small amount

  • of "tainted blood" to have an influence on a "pure-blooded" race.

  • Unsurprisingly, Nazi party leaders were quick to draw

  • some insane conclusions from Lorenz's behavioral studies

  • in the cause of what they called race hygiene.

  • Lorenz never denied his Nazi affiliation

  • but spent years trying to distance himself from the party

  • and apologizing for getting caught up in that evil.

  • Now how exactly does natural selection act

  • on behavior out there in the world?

  • That's where we turn to those two types of behavior

  • that are the only things in the world that matter:

  • eating and sex-having.

  • Behavior associated with finding and eating food

  • is known as foraging, which you've heard of,

  • and natural selection can act on behaviors that allow animals

  • to exploit food sources

  • while using the least amount of energy possible

  • this sweet spot is known as the optimal foraging model.

  • And the alligator snapping turtle has optimal foraging

  • all figured out.

  • Rather than running around hunting down its prey,

  • it simply sits in the water, and food comes to him.

  • See, the alligator snapping turtle has a long, pink tongue

  • divided into two segments, making it look like a tasty worm

  • to a passing fish.

  • In response to the stimulus of a passing fish,

  • it sticks out its tongue out and wiggles it.

  • Natural selection has, over many generations,

  • acted not only on turtles with pinker and more wiggly tongues

  • to catch more fish, it's also acted on those that best know how

  • and when to wiggle those tongues to get the most food.

  • So it's selecting both the physical trait

  • and the behavior that best exploits it.

  • And what could be sexier than a turtle's wiggly tongue dance?

  • Well, how about sex?

  • As we saw with our friend the horny Siberian hamster,

  • some behaviors and their associated physical features

  • are adapted to allow an animal to reproduce more,

  • simply by being better at finding mates.

  • But many times, animals of the same species live close together

  • or in groups, and determining who in what group gets to mate

  • creates some interesting behaviors and features.

  • This is what sexual selection, is all about.

  • Often, males of a species will find and defend a desirable habitat

  • to raise young in, and females will choose a male

  • based on their territory.

  • But what about those species, and there are many of them,

  • where the female picks a male not because of that,

  • but because of how he dances, or even weirder,

  • how much junk he's collected?

  • Take the male bower bird.

  • He builds an elaborate hut, or bower, out of twigs

  • and bits of grass, then spends an enormous amount of time

  • collecting stuff, sometimes piles of berries,

  • and sometimes piles of pretty, blue, plastic crap.

  • Ethologists believe that he's collecting the stuff

  • to attract the female to check out his elaborate house.

  • Once the female's been enticed to take a closer look,

  • the male starts to sing songs and dance around,

  • often mimicking other species, inside of his little house for her.

  • Females will inspect a number of these bowers

  • before choosing who to mate with.

  • Now, doing more complex dances and having more blue objects

  • in your bower scores bigger with females.

  • And ethologists have shown that a higher level of problem solving,

  • or intelligence, in males correlates

  • to both of these activities.

  • So yeah, it took some brawn to build that bower

  • and collect all that junk, but chicks also dig nerds

  • who can learn dances!

  • So the bowerbird's brain is evolving in response

  • to sexual selection by females.

  • This intelligence likely also translates

  • into other helpful behaviors like avoiding predators.

  • So thanks to the evolution of behavior,

  • we're really good at taking care of our nutritional and sexual needs.

  • But what's confused scientists for a long time

  • is why animals often look after others' needs.

  • For instance, vampire bats in South America will literally

  • regurgitate blood into the mouths of members of its clan

  • who didn't get a meal that night.

  • How do you explain animals who act altruistically like that?

  • We actually did a whole SciShow episode on this very subject

  • but basically, we can thank British scientist William Hamilton

  • for coming up with an equation to explain how natural selection

  • can simultaneously make animals fit and allow for

  • the evolution of altruism.

  • Hamilton found that the evolution of altruism

  • was best understood at the level of larger communities,

  • especially extended animal families.

  • Basically, altruism can evolve if the benefit of a behavior

  • is greater than its cost on an individual,

  • because it helped the individual's relatives enough

  • to make it worth it.

  • Hamilton called this inclusive fitness,

  • expanding Darwin's definition of fitness

  • basically, how many babies somebody's making

  • to include the offspring of relatives.

  • So I guess the only question left is,

  • if I forget to feed you two,

  • who is going to regurgitate blood into the other one's mouth?

  • Yeah, there's probably a reason that only happens with bats.

  • Thank you for watching this episode of Crash Course Biology.

  • Thank you to Cameo for being such a good kitty.

  • Yeah, she finally gets her treats.

  • There's a table of contents, of course.

  • If you want to reinforce any of the knowledge that you gained today.

  • If you have questions or ideas for us you can get in touch

  • with us on Facebook or Twitter, or of course, in the comments below.

  • We'll see you next time.

Behavior is action in response to a stimulus.

字幕與單字

單字即點即查 點擊單字可以查詢單字解釋

B2 中高級

動物行為學--生物速成班#25 (Animal Behavior - CrashCourse Biology #25)

  • 136 16
    Chi-feng Liu 發佈於 2021 年 01 月 14 日
影片單字